Chapter 11 Correspondence Relations: Imitation and RuleGoverned Behavior - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 53
About This Presentation
Title:

Chapter 11 Correspondence Relations: Imitation and RuleGoverned Behavior

Description:

... who are isolated from birth may show smaller effects of exposure to a model. ... protrusion, mouth opening, lip protrusion, and sequential finger movements. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:111
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 54
Provided by: michael449
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Chapter 11 Correspondence Relations: Imitation and RuleGoverned Behavior


1
Chapter 11Correspondence Relations Imitation
and Rule-Governed Behavior
2
Correspondence Relations
  • People often do what others do. A child who
    observes an older sibling raid the cookie jar may
    engage in similar behavior.
  • This is example involves correspondence relations
    between the demonstrated behavior and the
    replicated behavior.
  • Technically we may say that the behavior of one
    person sets the occasion for an equivalent
    response by the other.

3
Correspondence Relations Continued
  • There are other correspondence relations
    established by our culture. People look for and
    reinforce the correspondence between saying and
    doing.
  • A large part of socialization involves arranging
    social reinforcement for correspondence between
    what is said and what is done.

4
Correspondence Relations Continued
  • By the time a person is an adult, people expect
    consistency between spoken words and later
    performance. A minister who preaches moral
    conduct and lives a moral life is valued when
    moral words and moral deeds do not match, people
    become upset and act to correct the
    inconsistency. In such instances, what is said
    does not correspond adequately with what is done.

5
Imitation
  • Learning by observation involves doing what
    others do in which the performance of an
    observer or learner is regulated by the actions
    of a model. Although modeling can produce a
    variety of effects (e.g. social facilitation,
    stimulus enhancement, and so on), imitation
    requires that the learner emit a novel response
    that could only occur by observing a model emit a
    similar response.

6
Correspondence and Observational Learning
  • McDougall (1908) indicated that, as early as 4
    months of age, his child would stick out his
    tongue when an adult did the same.
  • Of course, 4-month-old infants already have a
    considerable history of interaction with their
    parents, and the observed behavior may simply be
    attributable to social conditioning. That is,
    people may smile and laugh when a young child
    imitates some adult movement.

7
Correspondence and Observational Learning
  • Presumably these social consequences strengthen
    imitation by the child. Although social
    conditioning is a possibility, recent research
    with newborn infants and animals suggests that
    innate or spontaneous imitation occurs without
    reinforcement.

8
Spontaneous Imitation
  • Innate or spontaneous imitation is based on
    evolution and natural selection rather than
    experiences during the lifetime of the
    individual. That is, imitation of others may be
    an important for of adaptive behavior.
  • Behavior may range from instinctive actions to
    more generalized set of responses, depending on
    the species.

9
Spontaneous Imitation
  • In addition, imitation may occur only when the
    model is present or it may be delayed for some
    time after the model has been removed.
  • Such delayed imitation is often as a more complex
    form since it involves remembering the modeled
    stimulus, rather than direct stimulus control.

10
Spontaneous Imitation
  • There are ethnological reports of imitation by
    animals and birds.
  • Birds
  • Japanese monkeys
  • Although it seems likely that some birds, monkeys
    and a few other species can imitate the novel
    responses of a model, these naturalistic studies
    are not sufficient to establish spontaneous
    imitation or to rule out alternative processes.

11
Imitation in the Laboratory
  • Thorndike (1911) conducted the earliest
    experiment on spontaneous imitation. The
    experiment involved getting out of a puzzle box
    by observing the successful performance of
    others.
  • The cat that observed the successful performance
    was no better at getting out of the box than
    other naïve animals.

12
Imitation in the Laboratory
  • Herbert and Harsh (1944) reported that cats could
    learn to solve manipulative problems by
    observation if they observed mistakes as well as
    successful performances.
  • Cats that observed both mistakes and correct
    responses by a model did better at problems than
    ones that only watched skillful performance.
  • When many alternative responses are available,
    seeing what does and does not work is necessary
    for observational learning.

13
Imitation in the Laboratory
  • Warden and his associates showed imitation in
    rhesus monkeys.
  • They trained monkeys by reinforcement to solve
    puzzles that opened doors to reveal hidden
    raisins. When this performance was well
    established, a naïve monkey watched a trained
    animal obtain raisins.
  • Observation of the model produced instantaneous
    solutions on 76 of the test trials. However,
    only the first instance of imitation could be
    described as spontaneous since the discovery of
    the raisin would reinforce this behavior and
    increase its likelihood.

14
Spontaneous and Delayed Imitation in Pigeons
  • In fact, it is difficult to find an experiment
    that reliably demonstrates spontaneous imitation.
    This is because reinforcement of the observers
    behavior always confounds the results.
  • Epstein, 1984.
  • Birds that strongly imitated the model were found
    to continue this imitation even when the model
    was removed (i.e. model-absent imitation). The
    data suggested that delayed-spontaneous imitation
    can occur in laboratory pigeons, but the results
    were inconsistent over subjects.
  • Spontaneous imitation occurred even after 24
    hours had elapsed between watching the model and
    the test for imitation.

15
Analysis of Epsteins Experiments
  • It therefore appears that spontaneous imitation
    is a real effect and is a form of phylogenetic
    behavior. That is, imitative behavior occurs
    because it has been important to the survival and
    reproduction of the species. In other words,
    organisms that imitated others were more likely
    to find food, avoid predators, and eventually
    produce offspring.

16
Analysis of Epsteins Experiments
  • The phylogenetic basis of spontaneous imitation
    is a reasonable hypothesis.
  • Pigeons who are isolated from birth may show
    smaller effects of exposure to a model. In
    addition, the effects of food reinforcement may
    have contributed to the results. Although
    observers were never directly reinforced with
    food for imitation, they did see the models eat
    from the feeder.

17
Spontaneous Imitation by Human Infants
  • There is evidence that spontaneous imitation
    occurs in human infants, almost from the moment
    of birth.
  • Meltzoff and Moore (1977) were the first to
    report that 12-to-21-day-old infants can imitate
    the facial and hand movements of adult models.
    In these experiments, the imitative responses
    were tongue protrusion, mouth opening, lip
    protrusion, and sequential finger movements.

18
Difficulties with Infant Imitation Research
  • The results of Meltzoff and Moores experiments
    remain controversial.
  • Infant imitation may simply be an artifact of the
    experimental procedures.
  • Other problems concern measurement, restricted
    range of responses, and infant attention to the
    modeled stimulus.

19
Difficulties with Infant Imitation Research
  • Meltzoff and Moore noted that infants responses
    are not discrete or well formed each time they
    occur.
  • This means that the response class is not
    clearly defined and it is therefore difficult to
    obtain a reliable measure. Because the response
    class is unclear, coders who observe the modeled
    gesture may see the imitative response more
    frequently than other response forms.

20
Delayed Imitation by Human Infants
  • A series of studies by Meltzoff (1988a, 1988b,
    1988c) indicate that infants ranging in age from
    9 to 24 months will imitate significantly more
    modeled actions than a control group over delays
    ranging from 24 hr in the youngest infants to 4
    months in the oldest infants.
  • Additional research indicates that 14-month-old
    infants will show delayed imitation of behavior
    modeled on television after 24 hr day.
  • In the same study, the researchers found delayed
    imitation by infants of behavior modeled by an
    expert toddler performing a novel response
    after a 48 hour and a change in content from the
    experimental situation to the home setting.

21
Analysis of Spontaneous and Delayed Imitation by
Infants
  • Spontaneous imitation in human newborns involves
    the infant observing a modeled gesture and
    responding with a set of muscle movements that
    correspond to the visual stimulus.
  • The correspondence between the modeled stimulus
    and the form of response is a remarkable
    achievement because the infant is unable to see
    its own face when it reproduces the facial
    gestures of the adult model.

22
Operant and Generalized Imitation
  • It is possible to train imitation as an operant
    in a social contingency of reinforcement.
  • The discriminative stimulus is the behavior of
    the model (SDmodel), the operant is a response
    that matches the modeled stimulus (Rmatch), and
    reinforcement is verbal praise (Srsocial).
  • Matching the model is reinforced, while
    noncorrespondent responses are extinguished.

23
Operant and Generalized Imitation
  • If imitation is reinforced and nonimitation is
    extinguished, imitation of the model will
    increase.
  • On the other hand, nonimitation will occur if
    imitation is extinguished and nonimitation is
    reinforced.

24
Operant and Generalized Imitation
  • Although, operant imitation provides a
    straightforward account to observational
    learning, Bandura (1969) noted that the operant
    account may be limited to situations in which the
    observer sees the model, an imitative response
    immediately occurs, and reinforcement follows.
    In everyday life, there are occasions when
    imitation does not conform to this sequence.

25
Operant and Generalized Imitation
  • Although Bandura (1969, 1977, 1986) has argued
    against an operant account based on these
    difficulties, Donald Baer and his associates
    provided a behavior analysis of imitation that
    handles each of the apparent challenges to the
    operant paradigm.
  • The approach is called generalized imitation and
    is based on operant principles of discrimination
    and generalization.

26
Generalized Imitation
  • The procedures of generalized imitation begin
    with simple reinforcement of correspondence
    between the modeled performance (SD model) and
    the imitative operant (Rmatch).
  • The actual discrimination procedures involve
    several modeled stimuli (SDs) and multiple
    operants (Rmatch).
  • In each case what the model does sets the
    occasion for reinforcement of a similar response
    by the child all other responses are
    extinguished. This training results in a
    stimulus class of modeled actions and an
    imitative response class. The child now imitates
    whichever of the three responses that the model
    performs.

27
Generalized Imitation
  • The next step is to test for generalization of
    the stimulus and response class.
  • Baer and Sherman (1964) showed that a new-modeled
    stimulus would set the occasion for a novel
    imitative response, without any further
    reinforcement.
  • If the puppet began pressing a lever, the child
    also imitated this performance even though this
    response was never reinforced with praise.
  • Generalized imitation account for the appearance
    of novel imitative acts in children- even when
    these responses were never reinforced.

28
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • Albert Bandura (1965) designed an experiment to
    show a form of learning by observation more
    complex than generalized imitation.
  • Children participated in this experiment on the
    imitation of aggressive behavior.
  • Each child watched a short film in which an adult
    demonstrated four distinctive aggressive actions
    toward an inflated Bobo doll.

29
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • Every aggressive action was accompanied by a
    unique verbal response. While sitting on the
    Bobo doll, the adult punched it in the face and
    said, Pow, right in the nose, boom, boom.
  • In another sequence, the adult hit the doll with
    a mallet saying Sockeroo, stay down.
  • Also, the model kicked the Bobo doll and said,
    Fly away, and threw rubber balls at the doll
    while saying, Bang.

30
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • Some of the children saw that model rewarded by
    another adult, who supplied soda, snack, and
    candies while saying, strong champion.
  • Other children saw the model receive negative
    consequences. The adult scolded and spanked the
    model for picking on that clown and warned him
    or her not to act that way again.
  • A third group saw that modeled aggression, but no
    consequences for the aggressive behavior were
    portrayed.

31
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • When the film ended, a child was taken to a room
    that contained many toys, including a Bobo doll.
    The child was encouraged to play with the toys
    and then was left alone.
  • Generally, there was a high frequency of
    imitative aggressive behavior toward the Bobo,
    and boys were more aggressive than girls.
  • Bandura (1965) also found that reward and
    punishment of the models actions affected the
    imitation of aggression.

32
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • Children who saw that model punished were less
    likely to imitate aggression than those who saw
    the model rewarded.
  • Children who saw the model rewarded did not
    differ in imitative aggression from those who
    watched the model perform the actions without
    receiving social consequences.
  • Importantly, this means that just seeing modeled
    aggression (no consequences) had about as much
    impact on the imitation as observing violence
    being rewarded.

33
The Bobo Doll Experiment
  • Finally, Bandura offered an incentive to all the
    children if they could remember the actions of
    the model in the film.
  • With this incentive, all three groups recalled
    the modeled aggression at about the same level.
  • It seemed that all children had learned equally
    from the modeled aggression, but those who
    witnessed punishment of the model were less
    inclined to perform the aggressive sequences.

34
Social Cognitive Interpretation
  • Bandura (1986) argued that the difference between
    learning and performing modeled aggression
    requires a cognitive theory of observational
    learning.
  • The observer pays attention to the modeled
    sequence, noting the arrangement of each action.
  • The general information in the sequence must be
    coded and rehearsed as when the child says,
    First sit on the Bobo, and then say the words
    pow. Once this abstract information is
    retained in memory, imitation is a matter of
    reproducing the component responses in the
    correct sequences.

35
Social Cognitive Interpretation
  • Complex behavior patterns, however, cannot be
    learned by observation until the component skills
    have been mastered.
  • Finally, the anticipated consequences of
    imitation determine whether an imitative response
    will occur.
  • People who expect positive outcomes are likely to
    perform actions they have witnessed, and those
    who expect negative consequences are less likely
    to imitate such actions.

36
Behavioral Interpretation
  • A behavioral interpretation for complex
    observational learning is that it may build on
    the processes of generalized imitation.
  • Generalized imitation provides an account of
    novel instances of imitation.
  • An operant perspective, imitation, is most likely
    to occur in situations in which It was reinforced
    previously.
  • Behavior is unlikely in situations in which it
    was extinguished, or in settings in which it was
    punished.

37
Behavioral Interpretation
  • This kind of conditioning history provides a
    plausible account of Banduras results concerning
    complex observational learning.
  • The learning and performance differences of the
    Bobo doll research may also be due to previous
    conditioning.
  • When Bandura offered an incentive for recalling
    the modeled action, he presented a discriminative
    stimulus that increased in probability of this
    verbal behavior.
  • For most children, it is likely that being
    promised a reward for recalling some action is a
    situation that has accompanied reinforcement in
    the past.

38
Rules, Observational Learning, and Self-Efficacy
  • Bandura has noted that observational learning in
    humans involves the discovery and use of abstract
    rules.
  • I began to develop the notion of modeling as a
    broad phenomenon that serves several functions.
    This conceptualization of modeling is concerned
    more with the observers extracting the rules and
    structure of behavior, rather than copying
    particular examples they had observed. Once they
    acquire that structure and the rules, they can
    use that knowledge to generate new patterns of
    behavior. Modeling is a much more complex
    abstract process than a simple process of
    response mimicry.

39
Rules, Observational Learning, and Self-Efficacy
  • From a behavioral perspective, extracting the
    rules is verbal operant behavior that describes
    the contingencies of reinforcement.
  • Skinner and Bandura agree about the importance of
    rules from human behavior, but they differ in
    terms of interpretation and philosophy.

40
Rules, Observational Learning, and Self-Efficacy
  • Bandura talks about rules as cognitive events,
    and Skinner views them as verbal descriptions.
  • For Skinner, following the rules is behavior
    under the control of verbal stimuli. That is,
    statements of rules, advice, maxims, or laws are
    discriminative stimuli that set the occasion for
    behavior.
  • Rules, as verbal descriptions, may affect
    observational learning.

41
Rules, Observational Learning, and Self-Efficacy
  • From a behavior analysis view, statements of
    self-efficacy, as a class of verbal stimuli, can
    affect subsequent behavior.
  • Based on conditioning for compliance, statements
    of self-efficacy often predict how a person will
    act in subsequent (similar) situations.

42
Rule-Governed Behavior
  • A large part of human behavior is regulated by
    verbal stimuli.
  • Stimuli are the products of speaking, writing,
    signing, and other forms.
  • The common property of these kinds of stimuli is
    that they describe the operating contingencies of
    reinforcement.
  • Formally, rules, instructions, advice, and laws
    are contingency-specifying stimuli, describing
    the SDR? Sr relations of everyday life.
  • The term rule-governed behavior is used when the
    listeners (readers) performance is regulated by
    contingency-specifying stimuli.

43
Constructing and Following Rules
  • In solving a problem, people often make up or
    construct their own discriminative stimuli.
  • A person who has an important, early morning
    appointment may set an alarm clock for six
    oclock a.m. Technically, setting the alarm is
    precurrent behavior, or an operant that precedes
    some other response. This behavior produces a
    discriminative stimulus that sets the occasion
    for getting up and going to the meeting.
  • Thus, a major function of precurrent behavior is
    the construction of SDs that regulate subsequent
    action.

44
Constructing and Following Rules
  • People also may construct discriminative stimuli
    through written words or spoken sounds.
  • For example, a person may make a shopping list
    before going to the supermarket. Making a list
    is precurrent behavior, and the list is a
    discriminative stimulus for choosing groceries.
  • Similarly, economical shoppers may say to
    themselves, only buy products that are on sale.
    This verbal stimulus acts something like the
    grocery list in the previous example.
  • As a rule, the verbal expression points to the
    relation between the stimuli, behavior, and
    reinforcement in the marketplace.

45
Rule-Governed and Contingency-Shaped Behavior
  • People are said to solve problems either by
    discovery or by instruction.
  • From a behavioral perspective the difference is
    between the direct effects of contingencies
    (discovery) and the indirect effects of rules
    (instruction).
  • When performance is attributed to direct exposure
    to reinforcement contingencies, behavior is said
    to be contingency-shaped.
  • As previously noted, performance set up by
    constructing and following instructions (and
    other verbal stimuli) is termed rule-governed
    behavior.

46
Rule-Governed and Contingency-Shaped Behavior
  • Although behavior attributed to rules and
    contingencies occasionally may look the same, the
    variables that affect performance are in fact
    quite different.
  • Reinforcement or following the advice of others
    in various situations may establish a general
    tendency to do what others recommend.
  • When directions are backed up with social
    punishment rather than natural consequences, they
    are called orders and commands. Individuals
    follow orders because they have been punished for
    disobedience.

47
Rule-Governed and Contingency-Shaped Behavior
  • Generalized obedience, however, may be a problem.
    Governments can induce blind obedience in which
    a person harms another without regard for moral
    consequences. In many countries, Amnesty
    International has documented the torture of
    political prisoners by guards and police. In
    these cases, obedience to authority is
    unquestioned and obviously results in serious
    harm or death to the victims.

48
Rule-Governed and Contingency-Shaped Behavior
  • The importance of reinforcement contingencies in
    establishing and maintaining rule-following is
    clearly seen with ineffective rules and
    instructions.
  • When rules describe delayed and improbable
    events, it is necessary to find other reasons to
    follow them.

49
Instructions and Contingencies
  • In his discussion of rule-governed and
    contingency-shaped behavior, Skinner (1969)
    speculated that instructions may affect
    performance differently than the actual
    contingencies of reinforcement.
  • One way to test this idea is to expose humans to
    reinforcement procedures that are accurately or
    inaccurately described by the experimenters
    instructions.
  • If behavior varies with the instructions while
    the actual contingencies remain the same, this
    would be evidence for Skinners assertion.

50
Instructions and Contingencies
  • An early study by Lippman and Meyer (1967) showed
    that human performance on a fixed-interval
    schedule varied with instructions.
  • In a similar study, Kaufman, Baron, and Kopp
    (1966) placed subjects on a variable-interval
    (VI) schedule of reinforcement and told them that
    points were available on either a fixed-interval
    or variable-ratio basis.
  • Performance was more in accord with the
    experimental instructions than with the actual VI
    contingencies.

51
Instructions and Contingencies
  • The fact that instructions, in these experiments,
    seem to override the actual contingencies has
    been used to argue against a reinforcement
    analysis of human behavior.
  • Bandura linked instructions to modeling and
    argued that both of these procedures activate
    subjects expectancies which, in turn, affect
    subsequent behavior. This means that expected
    reinforcement, rather than actual contingencies,
    is the stronger determinant.

52
Instructions and Contingencies
  • In addition, Spielberger and Denike (1966)
    disputed the claim that instructions were complex
    discriminative stimuli.
  • Both objections were addressed by Mark Galizio,
    the results of Galizios experiments provide
    strong support for the view that instructional
    control is a form of rule-governed behavior.

53
Rules as Function-Altering EventsAlerting
Discriminative Relations
  • Rules can act as function-altering events,
    altering the function of other stimuli and,
    thereby, the strength of relations among these
    stimuli and behavior.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com