Overview of the field of Environmental Epidemiology - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 124
About This Presentation
Title:

Overview of the field of Environmental Epidemiology

Description:

Overview of the field of Environmental Epidemiology Lydia B. Zablotska, MD, PhD Associate Professor Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:326
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 125
Provided by: Lyd653
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Overview of the field of Environmental Epidemiology


1
Overview of the field of Environmental
Epidemiology
  • Lydia B. Zablotska, MD, PhD
  • Associate Professor
  • Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics

2
Objectives
  • Review of study designs
  • How to choose a study design appropriate for a
    specific question
  • Exposure assessment
  • Dose modeling

3
Natural Progression in Epidemiologic Reasoning
  • 1st Suspicion that a factor influences disease
    occurrence. Arises from clinical practice, lab
    research, examining disease patterns by person,
    place and time, prior epidemiologic studies
  • 2nd Formulation of a specific hypothesis
  • 3rd Conduct epidemiologic study to determine
    the relationship between the exposure and the
    disease. Need to consider chance, bias,
    confounding when interpreting the study results.
  • 4th Judge whether association may be causal.
    Need to consider other research, strength of
    association, time directionality

4
Hypothesis Formation and Testing
  • Clues from many sources and imagination lead to
    hypothesis formation (inductive vs. deductive
    reasoning)
  • Conduct epidemiologic study to test hypothesis

5
Epidemiological Methods
0
  • Classifications by
  • approach to data collection
  • goal
  • timing and directionality
  • unit of analysis

6
Classification by approach to data collection
0
  • Experimental
  • RCTs, field trials, community intervention
  • and cluster randomized trials
  • Quasi-experimental
  • natural disaster studies
  • Non-experimental or observational
  • cohort, case-control, ecological

7
Classification by goal
0
  • Descriptive
  • ecological correlational studies, case reports,
    case series, cross-sectional surveys
  • Analytic
  • observational studies and intervention studies
    (RCTs)

8
Classification by timing and directionality
0
  • Directionality "Which did you observe first,
    the exposure or the disease?
  • forward (RCT, cohort)
  • backwards (case-control)
  • Timing Has the information being studied
    already occurred before the study actually
    began?"
  • retrospective and prospective cohort studies

9
Classification by timing and directionality
Retrospective cohort study
Diseased Non-diseased
exposed
Diseased Non-diseased
unexposed
x
past
present
future
Diseased Non-diseased
exposed
Diseased Non-diseased
unexposed
RCT
10
Classification by unit of analysis
  • What is a unit?
  • Observations for which outcome and exposure are
    measured
  • Individual-level variables are properties of
    individuals
  • ecological variables are properties of groups,
    organizations or places

11
Descriptive Epidemiology
  • Describe patterns of disease by person, place,
    and time
  • Person Who is getting the disease? (for example,
    what is their age, sex, religion, race,
    educational level etc?)

12
Mortality rates per 100,000 from diseases of the
heart by age and sex (2000)What hypotheses can
you generate from these data?
Age (in years) Men Women
25-34 10.3 5.5
35-44 41.6 17.2
45-54 142.7 50.3
55-64 378.6 160.4
65-74 909.2 479.9
75-84 2210.1 1501.5
85 6100.8 5740.1
13
Place
  • Where are the rates of disease the highest and
    lowest?
  • What hypotheses can you generate from this map?
  • Malignant Melanoma
    of Skin

14
Place
  • What hypotheses can you generate from this
    map?
  • Cancer of the Trachea, Bronchus and Lung

15
Variation on Place Migrant StudiesMortality
rates (per 100,000) due to stomach cancer. What
hypotheses can you generate from these data?
Japanese in Japan 58.4
Japanese Immigrants to California 29.9
Sons of Japanese Immigrants 11.7
Native Californians (Caucasians) 8.0
16
Time
  • Is the present frequency of disease different
    from the past?
  • What hypotheses can you generate from these data?

17
Main Epidemiologic Study Designs for Testing
Hypotheses
  • Experimental study
  • Cohort study
  • Case-control study
  • Each design represents a different way of
    harvesting information.
  • Selection of one over another depends on the
    particular research question, concerns of about
    data quality and efficiency, and practical and
    ethical considerations

18
  • Experimental study designs

19
  • Defining feature of experimental studies
    Investigator assigns exposure to study subjects
  • A) Experimental studies most closely resemble
    controlled laboratory experiments and serve as
    models for the conduct of observational studies.
  • B) They are the gold standard of epidemiological
    research. They have high status and validity, and
    can pick up small and modest effects

20
Ways to categorize experimental studies
  • Individual versus community treatment
  • allocated to individual OR entire community
  • Do women with stage I breast cancer given a
    lumpectomy alone survive as long without
    recurrence of disease as women given a lumpectomy
    plus radiation?
  • Does fluoride in the water supply decrease the
    frequency of dental caries in a community
    compared to a similar community without such
    water treatment?

21
Ways to categorize experimental studies
  • Preventive versus therapeutic prophylactic
    agent given to healthy or high-risk individual to
    prevent disease OR treatment given to diseased
    individual to reduce risk of recurrence, improve
    survival, quality of life
  • Does tamoxifen lower the incidence of breast
    cancer in women with high risk profile compared
    to high risk women not given tamoxifen?
  • Do combinations of two or three antiretroviral
    drugs prolong survival of AIDS patients as well
    as regimens of single drugs?

22
Population Hierarchy
23
Issues to be considered
  • A) Size, size, size - not just number of people
    in the trial, but how many endpoints (outcome
    under study) are expected
  • B) Restrictions on who is eligible (eligibility
    criteria)
  • Substantive What group are you interested in?
  • Logistics What group is accessible? Who will
    comply with study protocol? How feasible is
    complete and accurate follow-up on the subjects?
  • Characteristics of volunteers - How does study
    population differ from total experimental
    population?

24
Allocation of treatment
  • A) Should be random assignment
  • DEFINITION Each individual has the same chance
    of receiving each possible treatment
  • B) Some examples of random allocation
  • Random number table as each subject enrolled,
    assigned a number from the random number table
    assign even numbers to treatment A and odd to
    treatment B
  • Toss a coin for each subject headsA, tailsB
  • C) Some examples of nonrandom allocation
  • Alternate assignment of treatments
  • Assignment by day of the week

25
Allocation of treatment
  • D) Goal of randomization
  • To achieve baseline comparability between
    compared groups on factors related to outcome
  • Essence of good comparison between treatments
    is that the compared groups are the same EXCEPT
    for the treatment.
  • Any group of individuals will vary in response to
    a treatment based upon their sex, age, overall
    health, severity of illness - in short, any
    factor that is relevant to response to the
    treatment. The investigator knows some of these
    (like severity of illness), but there are many
    unknown factors that are also relevant.

26
Allocation of treatment
  • D) Goal of randomization
  • The compared groups should have the same
    distribution of all of these characteristics.
    That is what randomization can accomplish the
    equal distribution of known and unknown factors
    that are relevant to response to the treatment
    (confounders)
  • The larger the groups, the better randomization
    works

27
Use of placebo and blinding
  • A) Goals
  • Placebos are used to make the groups as
    comparable as possible (recall laboratory
    experiment)
  • Blinding subjects do not know if they are
    receiving treatment or placebo (single blind)
    neither subjects nor investigators know who is
    receiving treatment or placebo (double blind).
  • Purpose of blinding To avoid ascertainment bias,
    i.e. bias in ascertainment of outcome
  • Placebo allows study to be blind

28
Ascertaining the outcome
  • A) Goals
  • High follow-up rates dont lose people
  • Uniform follow-up for compared groups must be
    equally vigilant in follow-up in all compared
    groups
  • B) Penalty of non-uniform ascertainment of
    outcome is BIAS

29
Important issues in experimental studies
  • Ethical considerations
  • Equipoise Must be genuine doubt about efficacy
    of treatment yet sufficient belief that it may
    work
  • Stopping rules What if it becomes apparent,
    before the trial is over, that the new treatment
    is beneficial (and should not be withheld from
    the placebo group) or is toxic (and treatment
    should be withdrawn)?

30
Important issues in experimental studies
  • Planning for an informative result. If the
    study finds no difference between compared
    treatments, do you believe it? Or was there a
    difference but the study was not powerful enough
    to detect it? Initial consideration is study
    size.
  • Analyzing by intention to treat As the saying
    goes once randomized, always analyzed.

31
  • Cohort Studies

32
Principles of experimental studies applied to
observational cohort studies
  • 1. Randomization of treatment so groups are
    comparable on known and unknown confounders.
    Can't randomize in an observational study so
    select a comparison group as alike as possible to
    the exposed group
  •  
  •  

33
Principles of experimental studies applied to
observational cohort studies
  •  
  • 2. Use placebo in order to reduce bias. Cant
    use placebo in observational studies so you must
    make the groups as comparable as possible.
  •  

34
Principles of experimental studies applied to
observational cohort studies
  • 3. Blinding to avoid bias in outcome
    ascertainment.
  • In a cohort study, it is crucial to have high
    follow-up rates and comparable ascertainment of
    outcomes in the exposed and comparison groups.
  • You can blind the investigators conducting
    follow up and confirming the outcomes.

35
Timing of cohort studies
  • Retrospective both exposure and disease have
    occurred at start of study
  • Exposure------------------------?Disease

  • Study starts


36
Timing of cohort studies
  • Prospective exposure has (probably) occurred,
    disease has not occurred
  • Exposure----------------------?Disease
  • Study starts
  • Ambi-directional elements of both

37
Timing of cohort studies
  • How do you choose between a retrospective and a
    prospective design?
  • Retrospective
  • Cheaper, faster
  • Efficient with diseases with long latent period
  • Exposure data may be inadequate

38
Timing of cohort studies
  • How do you choose between a retrospective vs.
    prospective design?
  • Prospective
  • More expensive, time consuming
  • Not efficient for diseases with long latent
    periods
  • Better exposure and confounder data
  • Less vulnerable to bias

39
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Selection of exposed population
  • Choice depends upon hypothesis under study and
    feasibility considerations

40
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Examples of exposed populations
  • Occupational groups
  • Groups undergoing particular medical treatment
  • Groups with unusual dietary or life style factors
  • Professional groups (nurses, doctors)
  • Students or alumni of colleges
  • Geographically defined areas (e.g. Framingham)

41
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • For rare exposures, you need to assemble special
    cohorts (occupational groups, groups with unusual
    diets etc.)
  • Example of special cohort study
  • Rubber workers in Akron, Ohio
  • Exposure industrial solvent
  • Outcomes cancer

42
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • If exposure is common, you may want to use a
    general cohort that will facilitate accurate and
    complete ascertainment of data (Doctors, nurses,
    well-defined communities)

43
  • Example of general cohort study
  • Framingham Study
  • Exposures smoking, hypertension, family history
  • Outcomes heart disease, stroke, gout, etc.

44
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Selection of comparison (unexposed) group
  • Principle You want the comparison (unexposed)
    group to be as similar as possible to the exposed
    group with respect to all other factors except
    the exposure. If the exposure has no effect on
    disease occurrence, then the rate of disease in
    the exposed and comparison groups will be the
    same.

45
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Selection of comparison (unexposed) group
    (contd)
  • Counterfactual ideal The ideal comparison group
    consists of exactly the same individuals in the
    exposed group had they not been exposed. Since it
    is impossible for the same person to be exposed
    and unexposed simultaneously, epidemiologists
    much select different sets of people who are as
    similar as possible.

46
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Three possible sources of comparison group
  • 1. Internal comparison unexposed members of same
    cohort
  • Ex Framingham study

47
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Three possible sources of comparison group
  • 2. Comparison cohort a cohort who is not
    exposed from another similar population
  • Ex Asbestos textile vs. cotton textile workers

48
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • 3. General population data Use pre-existing
    data from the general population as the basis for
    comparison. General population is commonly used
    in occupational studies. Usually find healthy
    worker effect
  • Ex. A study of asbestos and lung cancer with
    U.S. male population as the comparison group

49
Which of the three comparison groups is best?
50
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Sources of exposure information
  • Pre-existing records - inexpensive, data
    recorded before disease occurrence but level of
    detail may be inadequate. Also, records may be
    missing, usually don't contain information on
    confounders

51
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Sources of exposure information
  • Questionnaires, interviews good for information
    not routinely recorded but have potential for
    recall bias
  • Direct physical exams, tests, environmental
    monitoring may be needed to ascertain certain
    exposures.

52
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Sources of outcome information
  • Death certificates
  • Physician, hospital, health plan records
  • Questionnaires (verify by records)
  • Medical exams

53
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Goal is to obtain complete follow-up information
    on all subjects regardless of exposure status.
    You can use blinding (like an experimental study)
    to ensure that there is comparable ascertainment
    of the outcome in both groups.

54
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Approaches to follow-up
  • In any cohort study, the ascertainment of outcome
    data involves tracing or following all subjects
    from exposure into the future.

55
Issues in design of cohort studies
  • Approaches to follow-up
  • Resources utilized to conduct follow-up town
    lists, Polk directories, telephone books birth,
    death, marriage records driver's license lists,
    physician and hospital records relatives,
    friends.
  • This is a time consuming process but high losses
    to follow-up raise doubts about validity of study

56
Ex. Tuberculosis treatment and breast cancer
study
57
Classifying Person-Time
  • Each unit of person-time contributed by an
    individual has its own exposure classification
  • Must consider the etiologically relevant exposure
  • Exposure may change over time

Exposure
Disease Initiation
Disease Detection
Latent period
Induction period
58
Classifying Person-Time cont.
  • Time at which exposure occurs ? time at risk of
    exposure effects
  • Radiation from an atomic bomb and risk of cancer
  • Only the time at risk for exposure effects should
    be counted in the denominator of the incidence
    rate for that level of exposure
  • If the induction time is not known, can estimate
    empirically by calculating the incidence rates
    for differing categories of time since exposure

59
Classifying Person-Time cont.
  • How do you classify person-time contributed by
    exposed subjects before the minimum induction
    time has elapsed or after the maximum induction
    time has passed?
  • Example
  • Exposure Rotavirus vaccine
  • Outcome Intussusception
  • Assume induction period ranges from 1-7 days

Exposure
Disease Initiation
Induction period
60
Classifying Exposure
  • Exposure may change over time
  • Ideally, measure exposure constantly and classify
    each unit of person-time
  • A given individual can contribute person-time to
    one or more exposure category in the same study!
  • More often, assume one measure of exposure
    history is the only aspect of exposure associated
    with current disease risk
  • Current, average, cumulative, etc.
  • Lag exposure to account for induction time
    between exposure and disease initiation

61
Analysis of cohort studies
  • Basic analysis involves calculation of incidence
    of disease among exposed and unexposed groups.
  • Depending on available data, you can calculate
    cumulative incidence or incidence rates.
  • Recall set up of 2 x 2 tables.

62
Analysis of cohort studies
  • Example Tuberculosis treatment and breast cancer
    study
  • Followed 1,047 women who were treated with air
    collapse therapy and exposed to numerous
    fluoroscopic examinations (radiation) and 717 who
    received other treatments. A total of 47,036
    woman-years of follow-up were accumulated during
    which 56 breast cancer cases occurred.

63
Analysis of cohort studies
Breast Cancer Cases Woman-Years of follow-up
Exposed 41 28,001
Unexposed 15 19,025
Total 56 47,036
IR1 41/28,011 1.5/1,000 woman-years IR0
15/19,025 0.8/1,000 woman-years RR IR1/IR0
1.9 Interpretation Women exposed to
fluoroscopies had 1.9 times the risk of breast
cancer compared to unexposed women.
64
Strengths of Cohort Studies
  • Efficient for rare exposures, diseases with long
    induction and latent period
  • Can evaluate multiple effects of an exposure
  • If prospective, good information on exposures,
    less vulnerable to bias, and clear temporal
    relationship between exposure and disease

65
Weaknesses of Cohort Studies
  • Inefficient for rare outcomes
  • If retrospective, poor information on exposure
    and other key variables, more vulnerable to bias
  • If prospective, expensive and time consuming,
    inefficient for diseases with long induction and
    latent period
  • Keep these strengths and weaknesses in mind for
    comparison with case-control studies

66
  • Case-control studies

67
TROHOC STUDIES
  • This disparaging term was given to case-control
    studies because their logic seemed backwards
    (trohoc is ?? spelled backwards) and they seemed
    more prone to bias than other designs.
  • No basis for this derogation.
  • Case-control studies are a logical extension of
    cohort studies and an efficient way to learn
    about associations.

68
General Definition of a Case-Control Study A
method of sampling a population in which cases of
disease are identified and enrolled, and a sample
of the population that produced the cases is
identified and enrolled. Exposures are
determined for individuals in each group.
69
When is it desirable to conduct a case-control
study?
  • When exposure data are expensive or difficult to
    obtain
  • - Ex Pesticide and breast cancer study
  • When disease has long induction and latent period
  • - Ex Cancer, cardiovascular disease
  • When the disease is rare
  • Ex Studying risk factors for birth defects
  • When little is known about the disease
  • Ex. Early studies of AIDS
  • When underlying population is dynamic
  • Ex Studying breast cancer on Cape Cod

70
Cases
  • Criteria for case definition should lead to
    accurate classification of disease
  • Efficient and accurate sources should be used to
    identify cases existing registries, hospitals
  • What do the cases give you? Think of the
    standard 2 X 2 table

Disease
Yes (case) No Total
Yes a ? ?
No c ? ?
Total ac ? ?
Exposed
71
Cases give you the numerators of the rates of
disease in exposed and unexposed groups being
compared
  • Rate of disease in exposed a/?
  • Rate of disease in unexposed c/?

What is missing? The denominators! If this were
a cohort study, you would have the total
population (if you were calculating cumulative
incidence) or total person-years (if you were
calculating incidence rates) for both the
exposed and non exposed groups, which would
provide the denominators for the compared rates.
72
Where do you get the information for the
denominators in a case control study? THE
CONTROLS.
  • A case-control study can be considered a more
    efficient form of a cohort study.
  • Cases are the same as those that would be
    included in a cohort study.
  • Controls provide a fast and inexpensive means of
    obtaining the exposure experience in the
    population that gave rise to the cases.

73
Controls
  • Definition A sample of the source population
    that gave rise to the cases.
  • Purpose To estimate the exposure distribution in
    the source population that produced the cases.

74
Selecting Controls
  • Advantages of general population controls
  • Because of selection process, investigator is
    usually assured that they come from the same base
    population as the cases.
  • Disadvantages of general population controls
  • Time consuming, expensive, hard to contact and
    get cooperation may remember exposures
    differently than cases

75
Hospital-Based Controls cont.
  • Limit diagnoses for controls to conditions with
    no association with the exposure
  • May exclude most potential controls
  • Exclusion criteria only applies to the cause of
    the current hospitalization
  • Reasonable to exclude categories of potential
    controls on the suspicion that a given category
    might be related to exposure
  • Imprudent to use only a single diagnostic
    category as a source of controls

76
Deceased Controls
  • Not members of the source population for the
    cases
  • If exposure is associated with mortality, dead
    controls will misrepresent exposure distribution
    in source population
  • Even if cases are dead, generally better to
    choose living controls
  • Do not need a proxy interview for living controls
    of dead cases

77
Comparability of Information
  • Comparability of information is often used to
    guide control selection and data collection
  • BUT
  • Non-differential exposure measurement error does
    not guarantee that bias will be toward the null
  • Efforts to ensure equal accuracy of exposure data
    tend to produce equal accuracy of data on other
    variables
  • Overall bias due to non-differential error in
    confounders and effect modifiers can be larger
    than error produced by unequal accuracy of
    exposure data from cases and controls

78
Selecting Controls
  • Advantages of hospital controls
  • Same selection factors that led cases to hospital
    led controls to hospital
  • Easily identifiable and accessible (so less
    expensive than population-based controls)
  • Accuracy of exposure recall comparable to that of
    cases since controls are also sick
  • More willing to participate than population-based
    controls

79
Selecting Controls
  • Disadvantages of hospital controls
  • Since hospital based controls are ill, they may
    not accurately represent the exposure history in
    the population that produced the cases
  • Hospital catchment areas may be different for
    different diseases

80
Selecting Controls
  • Special control groups like friends, spouses,
    siblings, and deceased individuals.
  • These special controls are rarely used.
  • Cases not be able to nominate controls because
    they have few appropriate friends, are widowed or
    are only or adopted children.
  • Dead controls are tricky to use because they are
    more likely than living controls to smoke and
    drink.

81
Friend/Family Controls
  • Being named as a friend control may be related to
    exposure
  • Reclusive people are less likely to be named
  • Investigator dependent on cases for identifying
    controls
  • Friend groups often overlap, so persons with more
    friends are more likely to be selected as a
    control

82
Neighborhood Controls
  • Sample residences
  • May individually match cases to one or more
    controls residing in the same neighborhood
  • If neighborhood is associated with exposure, must
    control for matching in the analysis
  • Neighbors may not be the source population of the
    cases
  • Cases at a VA hospital

83
Random Digit Dialing
  • Case eligibility should include residence in a
    house with a telephone
  • Probability of calling a number ? probability of
    contacting an eligible control
  • Households vary in the number of people, amount
    of time a person is at home, and the number of
    operating phones
  • Method requires a great deal of time and labor

84
Random Digit Dialing cont.
  • Answering machines, voicemail, and caller ID
    reduce response rates
  • Cell phones reduce validity of assuming source
    population can be randomly sampled using this
    method
  • Recent CDC survey showed 2 increase in binge
    drinking compared to 2009 data more cell phone
    numbers included, and average age of respondents
    decreased
  • May not be able to distinguish business and
    residential numbers - difficult to estimate
    proportion of non-responders

85
Control Sampling Schemes
Control Sampling Method Description Measure of effect estimated by the OR
Case-cohort Persons at risk of disease at baseline Risk ratio Rate ratio
Density sampling Proportional to person-time accumulated by persons at risk of disease during follow-up Rate Ratio
Cumulative case-control Persons at risk of disease who are non-cases at the end of follow-up Incidence Odds Ratio Risk Ratio
Only need rare disease assumption when
estimating the risk ratio from the odds ratio.
86
Density Sampling
  • Sample controls at a steady rate per unit time
    over period in which cases are sampled
  • Probability of being selected as a control is
    proportional to amount of time person spends at
    risk of disease in source population
  • Individual may be selected as a control while
    they are at risk for disease, and subsequently
    become a case
  • Incidence density sampling or risk set sampling
    is a form of density sampling in which you match
    cases and controls on time

87
Variations in case-control study designs
  • Case-cohort
  • Nested case-control
  • Case-control studies without controls
  • Traditional case series
  • Case-crossover
  • Case-specular

88
Sampling a cohort population for controls nested
case-control study
  • 1. Sample the population at risk at the start of
    the observation period
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ------------------------
  • Start FU
    End FU
  • 2. Sample population at risk as cases develop
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ------------------------
  • Start FU
    End FU

  • 3. Sample survivors at the end of the observation
    period
  • -------------------------------------------------
    -----------------------
  • Start FU
    End FU


89
Strengths case-control studies
  • Efficient for rare diseases and diseases with
    long induction and latent period.
  • Can evaluate many risk factors for the same
    disease so good for diseases about which little
    is known

90
Weaknesses of case-control studies
  • Inefficient for rare exposures
  • Vulnerable to bias because of retrospective
    nature of study
  • May have poor information on exposure because
    retrospective
  • Difficult to infer temporal relationship between
    exposure and disease
  • How do these strengths and weaknesses
    compare to cohort studies?

91
Comparisons between Case-control and Cohort study
design
Characteristics Case-control Cohort study
Select subjects based on Disease status Exposure Status
Exposure good for common exposures Good for rare exposures
Cost-effectiveness Cheaper and less time consuming Expensive and time consuming
Disease Frequency Good for rare diseases Good for common diseases
Establish temporal order Temporality generally not clear Temporality generally clear
Incidence calculation Can not calculate incidence/risk/rate Can calculate incidence risk or rate depending on study design
Study more than one outcome No Yes
Examine gt1 exposure Yes Generally no
Inherent Study Selection problem Difficult to ascertain appropriate control group Not applicable since start with a source population
Subject to biases Susceptible to more biases Particularly recall bias Less subject to biases-except to loss to follow-up (Loss of subjects due to migration, lack of participation, withdrawal death)
92
Exposure Classification
  • Same principles as discussed for cohort studies
  • Cases exposure should be classified as of the
    time of diagnosis or disease onset, accounting
    for induction time hypotheses
  • Controls should be classified according to their
    exposure status at the time of selection,
    accounting for induction time hypotheses

93
Timing of Exposure Classification
  • Selection time does not necessarily refer to the
    time at which a control is first identified
  • For hospital-based controls, selection time may
    be date of diagnosis for the disease that
    resulted in the current hospitalization
  • Date of interview is often used if there is not
    an event analogous to the cases date of
    diagnosis
  • Interviewers should be blinded to case-control
    status whenever possible

94
  • Ecological studies

95
Main properties of ecological studies
0
  • Units of analysis are groups
  • Both exposure and outcome are measured for groups

96
Measures of exposure in ecological studies
  • Aggregate summaries of observations derived
    from individuals in each group
  • the proportion of smokers and median family
    income
  • proportion of the population under the age of 18
    and rate of thyroid cancer

97
Measures of exposure in ecological studies
  • Aggregate summaries of observations derived
    from individuals in each group)
  • Environmental physical characteristics of the
    place in which members of each group live or
    work with an analog at the individual level
  • air pollution level and hours of sunlight
  • well water arsenic concentration and skin lesion
    rate in each village in Bangladesh

98
Measures of exposure in ecological studies
  • Aggregate summaries of observations derived
    from individuals in each group)
  • Environmental physical characteristics of the
    place in which members of each group live or
    work with an analog at the individual level
  • Global attributes of groups, organizations or
    places for which there is no distinct analogue at
    the individual level
  • population density
  • existence of special law or type of health-care
    system

99
0
  • Measure of association is correlation
    coefficient, r
  • Quantifies the extent to which two variables
    (exposure and outcome) are associated
  • r varies between 1 and 1

100
If association is linear
0
  • y b0 b1x, where b1 is slope (regression
    coefficient)
  • Proportionate increase or decrease in disease
    frequency for every unit change in level of
    exposure

101
Examples of ecological studies
0
  • Exploratory studies
  • Multiple-group studies
  • differences among groups
  • Time-trend studies
  • changes over time within groups
  • Mixed studies
  • combination of the above

102
Example of exploratory ecological study
0
Cotterill et al., (2001) Eur J Cancer 37
1020-26.
103
0
104
Example of multi-group ecological study
0
Prisyazhniuk et al., Lancet (1991) 338 1334-35.
105
Strengths of ecological studies
0
  • Low cost and convenience
  • Examples of secondary data sources population
    registries, vital records, large surveys
  • Ability to overcome measurement limitations of
    individual-level studies
  • When exposures cannot be measured accurately for
    large numbers of subjects
  • When there is too much within-person variability
    in exposures (e.g., dietary factors)
  • Ability to overcome design limitations of
    individual-level studies
  • When there is not enough variability within the
    study area

106
Limitations of ecological studies
0
  • No information on the cross-classification of
    exposures and outcomes within groups
  • Lack of ability to control for the effects of
    possible confounding variables
  • Exposure can be associated with a number of
    factors that are related to the elevated risk of
    disease it is not possible to separate their
    effects using ecological data

A B
C D
107
Limitations of ecological studies, continued
0
  • Unclear temporality
  • we do not know temporality at the individual
    level
  • Ecological variables do not measure the same
    thing as individual variables with the same name
  • Example
  • Association between individual-level income and
    mortality
  • Association between country-level income and
    mortality
  • Data collected for other purposes
  • Ecological bias

108
  • Ecological study of use of oral contraceptives in
    the U.S. and risk of CHD in 1950-76 (Rosenberg,
    1979)
  • Findings NO association between OC use and risk
    of fatal CHD

0
Annual mortality from CHD 800,000
Historical trend while use of OC increased, the
risk of CHD among women of childbearing age
decreased by 30
18,000 among women of childbearing age
12,600 CHD deaths
decrease during 1950-76
109
0
  • Analytical studies of use of oral contraceptives
    in the U.S. and risk of fatal CHD
  • Findings a two-fold increase in risk of fatal
    CHD among OC users compared with non-users

400 increase in CHD deaths attributable to OC
use
110
Ecological fallacy
0
  • At the group level
  • No relationship between OC use and CHD mortality
    in young women
  • At the individual level
  • two-fold increase in risk of CHD among OC users
    compared to nonusers
  • Summary
  • Impossible to detect from correlational data
  • Incorrect to assume that no relationship between
    OC use and CHD mortality

111
Ecological fallacy
0
  • Fallacy of drawing inferences regarding
    associations at the individual level based on the
    group-level data
  • The group-level data
  • inverse linear relationship between alcohol
    consumption and CHD mortality
  • Those who consume large quantities of alcohol
    have the smallest mortality

Group level
Individual level
112
Ecological fallacy, continued
0
  • This does not mean that every ecological study
    has ecological fallacy!
  • The importance of the ecological fallacy may
    differ for different research questions
  • Potential strategies to reduce ecological
    fallacy
  • Use smaller units to make groups more homogeneous
  • Supplement ecological variables with
    individual-level variables

113
0
Atomistic fallacy
  • drawing inferences at a higher level from
    analyses performed at a lower level
  • Example
  • in a case-control collect information on various
    possible exposures but ignore the geographic,
    spatial, and social context in which a person
    lives

Group level
Individual level
114
0
  • Example
  • Infant mortality is influenced by
  • Individual-level characteristics
  • Maternal factors
  • genes
  • maternal nutrition
  • habits
  • Community-level variables
  • Contextual factors
  • environmental pollution
  • geographical distance to a health care facility
  • housing costs
  • age of housing
  • availability of social support

115
Which study design to choose?
  • In theory, it's possible to use each design to
    test a hypothesis
  • Example Suppose you want to study the
    relationship between dietary Vitamin A and lung
    cancer.

116
Cohort Study Option
  • Subjects are chosen on the basis of exposure
    status and followed to assess the occurrence of
    disease
  • High Vitamin A consumption ---------------gt lung
    cancer or not
  • Low Vitamin A Consumption --------------gt lung
    cancer or not
  • What are the advantages and disadvantages of this
    option?

117
Experimental Study Option
  • Special type of cohort study in which
    investigator assigns the exposure to individuals,
    preferably at random
  •  
  • Investigator assigns exposure to
  • High Vit A consumption ----------------gt lung
    cancer or not
  • Low Vit A consumption ----------------gt lung
    cancer or not
  • What are the advantages and disadvantages of this
    option?

118
Case-Control Study Option
  • Cases with the disease and controls who generally
    do not have the disease are chosen and past
    exposure to a factor is determined
  • Prior Vitamin A consumption lt----------- lung
    cancer cases
  • Prior Vitamin A consumption lt---------- controls
  • What are the advantages and disadvantages of this
    option?

119
In practice, choice of study design depends on
  • State of knowledge
  • Frequency of exposure and disease
  • Time, cost and other feasibility considerations
  • Each study design has unique and complementary
    advantages and disadvantages

120
Exposure assessment
  • Most environmental exposures are complex,
    time-varying
  • Relevant concepts dose, burden, markers
  • Example
  • Absorbed dose amount of energy imparted to the
    mass of exposed body or organ
  • Equivalent dose absorbed dose multiplied by the
    radiation weighting factor used to compare
    different types of radiation
  • Effective dose equivalent dose averaged over
    all organs used in biomonitoring

121
Exposure assessment
122
Exposure-dose relations
  • Uptake (losses associated with absorption)
  • Clearance
  • Compartmentalization
  • Development of the dosimetric models
  • Development of model structure
  • Estimation of model parameters
  • Validation and testing of the model, including
    sensitivity analyses

123
Advantages and limitations of dose modeling
  • Improve study validity and precision by weighting
    exposure data in a way that improves the fit of
    epi models
  • Helpful in extrapolating results
  • Require specific assumptions about the structure
    of the dose model and the values of its
    parameters
  • Uncertainties in exposure measurements may
    exacerbate problems
  • Shifts attention from environmental quantity to a
    physiologic one

124
Dose-response relations
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com