Fitting Marginal Structural Models - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Fitting Marginal Structural Models

Description:

Robins JM, Hernan MA, Brumback B. Marginal structural models and causal ... Bang H, Robins JM (2005). Doubly Robust Estimation in Missing Data and Causal ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:234
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 44
Provided by: eleanorpul
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Fitting Marginal Structural Models


1
Fitting Marginal Structural Models
  • Eleanor M Pullenayegum
  • Asst Professor
  • Dept of Clin. Epi Biostatistics
  • pullena_at_mcmaster.ca

2
Outline
  • Causality and observational data
  • Inverse-Probability weighting and MSMs
  • Fitting an MSM
  • Goodness-of-fit
  • Assumptions/ Interpretation

3
Causality in Medical Research
  • Often want to establish a causal association
    between a treatment/exposure and an event
  • Difficult to do with observational data due to
    confounding
  • Gold-standard for causal inferences is the
    randomized trial
  • Randomize half the patients to receive the
    treatment/exposure, and half to receive usual
    care
  • Deals with measured and unmeasured confounders

4
Randomized trials are not always possible
  • Sometimes, they are unethical
  • cannot do a randomized trial on the effects of
    second-hand smoke on lung cancer
  • or a randomized trial of the effects of living
    near power stations
  • Sometimes, they are not feasible
  • Study of a rare disease (funding is an issue)

5
Observational Studies
  • Observe rather than experiment (or interfere!)
  • Recruit some people who are exposed to
    second-hand smoke and some who are not
  • Study communities living close to power lines vs.
    those who dont
  • Confounding is a major concern
  • For 1st example, are workplace environment, home
    environment, age, gender, income similar between
    exposed and unexposed?
  • For 2nd example, are education, family income,
    air pollution similar between cases and controls?

6
Handling Confounding
  • Match exposed and unexposed on key confounders
  • e.g. for every family living close to a power
    station, attempt to find a control family living
    in a similar neighbourhood with a similar income
  • Adjust for confounders
  • for the smoking example, adjust for age, gender,
    level of education, income, type of work, family
    history of cancer etc.
  • Cannot deal with unmeasured confounders

7
Causal Pathways
  • There are some things we cannot adjust for
  • When studying the effect of a lipid-lowering drug
    on heart disease, we cant adjust for
    LDL-cholesterol level

8
Causal Pathways
Drug
LDL-cholesterol
Heart Disease
LDL-cholesterol mediates the effect of the
drug Cannot adjust for variables that are on the
causal pathway between exposure and outcome.
9
Motivating Example
  • Juvenile Dermatomyositis (JDM) is a rare but
    serious skin/muscle disease in children
  • Standard treatment is with steroids (Prednisone),
    however these have unpleasant side-effects
  • Intravenous immunoglobulin (IVIg) is a possible
    alternative treatment
  • DAS measures disease activity

10
JDM Dataset
  • 81 kids, 7 on IVIg at baseline, 23 on IVIG later
  • Outcome is time to quiescence
  • Quiescence happens when DAS0
  • IVIg tends to be given when the child is doing
    particularly badly (high DAS)
  • DAS is a counfounder

11
Causal Pathway for JDM study

DASt
DASt1

IVIgt
IVIgt1
Time-to-Quiescence
  • DAS confounds IVIg and outcome
  • DAS is on the causal pathway

12
A Thought Experiment
  • Suppose that at each time t, we could create an
    identical copy of each child i.
  • Then if the real child received IVIG, we would
    give the copy control and vice versa
  • We could then compare the child to its copy
  • Solves confounding by matching the child is
    matched with the copy
  • If treatment varies on a monthly basis and we
    follow for 5 years, we would have 260-1 copies

13
Counterfactuals
  • Clearly, this is impossible.
  • But we can use the idea
  • Define the counterfactuals for child i to be the
    outcomes for each of the 260-1 imaginary copies
  • Idea treat the counterfactuals as missing data

14
Inverse-Probability Weighting
  • Inverse-Probability Weighting (IPW) is a way of
    re-weighting the dataset to account for selective
    observation
  • E.g. if we have missing data, then we weight the
    observed data by the inverse of the probability
    of being observed
  • Why does this work?
  • Suppose we have a response Yij, treatment
    indicator xij and Rij1 if Yij observed, 0 o/w

15
Inverse-Probability Weighting
  • Suppose we want to fit the marginal model
  • Usually, we solve the GEE equation
  • If we use just the observed data, we solve
  • LHS does not have mean 0

16
Inverse-Probability Weighting
  • If we replace D by with
    pij, the conditional probability of observing
    Yij, then
  • What to condition on?
  • Must condition on Yij
  • If MAR, then conditionally independent given
    previous Y

17
Marginal Structural Models
  • MSMs use inverse-probability weighting to deal
    with the unobserved (missing) counterfactuals
  • We cannot adjust for confounders
  • but using IPW, can re-weight the dataset so that
    treatment and covariates are unconfounded
  • i.e. mean covariate levels are the sample between
    treated and untreated patients
  • So can do a simple marginal analysis

18
Probability-of-Treatment Model
  • Weighting is based on the Probability-of-Treatment
    model
  • Treatment is longitudinal
  • For each child at each time, need probability of
    receiving the observed treatment trajectory
  • Probability is conditional on past responses and
    confounders
  • Assume independent of current response

19
JDM Example
  • Probability of being on IVIg at baseline
    (logistic regression)
  • Probability of transitioning onto IVIg (Cox PH)
  • Probability of transitioning off IVIg (Cox PH)
  • Suppose a child initiates IVIG at 8 months and is
    still on IVIG at 12 months.
  • What is the probability of the observed treatment
    pattern?

20
Trratment probability
P(transition at month 8)
P(no transition before month 8)
P(no transition off before month 12)
P(not on IVIg at baseline)
0
8
12
No IVIg
Initiate IVIg
Still on IVIG
21
Model Fitting
  • First identified covariates univariately
  • Then entered those that were sig. into model and
    refined (by removing those that were no longer
    sig.)
  • IVIg at baseline Functional status (any vs.
    none) OR 11.6, 95 CI 1.94 to 69.7 abnormal
    swallow/voice OR 6.28, 95 CI 0.983 to 4.02.
  • IVIg termination no covariates

22
Assessing goodness-of-fit
  • If the IPT weights are correct, in the
    re-weighted population, treatment and covariates
    are unconfounded
  • This property is
  • crucial
  • testable
  • so we should test it!

23
Goodness-of-fit in the JDM study
  • Biggest concern is that kids are doing badly when
    they start IVIg
  • If inverse-probability weights are correct, then
    at each time t, amongst patients previously
    IVIg-naïve, IVIg is not associated with
    covariates.
  • Will look at differences in mean covariate values
    by current IVIg status amongst patients
    previously IVIg-naïve
  • Data are longitudinal, so use a GEE analysis,
    adjusting for time

24
Model 1 HRs for Treatment Initiation
Hazard Ratios and 95 confidence intervals for
initiating treatment
25
(No Transcript)
26
Model 2 -Revised Treatment initiation
Hazard Ratios and 95 confidence intervals for
initiating treatment
27
New goodness-of-fit
28
Back to basics
  • Some patients start IVIg because they are
    steroid-resistant (early-starters)
  • Others start because they are steroid-dependent
    (late-starters)
  • Repeat model-fitting process separately for early
    and late starters

29
(No Transcript)
30
Refined two-stage model
31
(No Transcript)
32
Efficacy Results
33
Other concerns with MSMs
  • Format of treatment effect (e.g. constant over
    time, PH etc.)
  • Unmeasured counfounders
  • Lack of efficiency
  • Experimental Treatment Assignment

34
Efficiency
  • IPW reduces bias but also reduces efficiency
  • The further the weights are from 1, the worse the
    efficiency
  • Can stabilise the weights
  • Estimating equations will still be zero-mean if
    we multiply Dijj by a factor depending on j and
    treatment
  • In JDM study, we used
  • DijjRijP(Rx history)/P(Rx historyconfounders)

35
Efficiency other techniques
  • Doubly robust methods (Bang Robins)
  • Could have used a more information-rich outcome
  • Did a secondary analysis using DAS as the outcome
    got far more precise (and more positive) results

36
Experimental Treatment Assignment
  • In order for MSMs to work, there must be some
    experimentality in the way treatment is assigned
  • Intuitively, if we can predict perfectly who will
    get what treatment, then we have complete
    confounding
  • Mathematically, if pij is 0 then were in
    trouble!
  • Actually, we get into trouble if pij 0 or 1

37
Testing the ETA simple checks
  • At each time j, review the distribution of
    covariates amongst those who are on treatment vs.
    those who are not.
  • Review the distribution of the weights
  • check p bounded away from 0/1
  • In the JDM example, also check distn of
    transition probabilities

38
Testing the ETA more advanced methods
  • Bootstrapping
  • Wang Y, Petersen ML, Bangsberg D, van der Laan
    MJ. Diagnosing bias in the inverse probability of
    treatment weighted estimator resulting from
    violation of experimental treatment assignment.
    UC Berkeley Division of Biostatistics working
    paper series, 2006.

39
Implementing MSMs
  • For time-to-event outcome, can do weighted PH
    regression in R
  • Used the svycoxph function from the survey
    package
  • For continuous (or binary) outcome, use weighted
    GEE
  • Used proc genmod in SAS with scgwt
  • Weighted GEEs are not straightforward in R
  • STATA could probably handle either type of outcome

40
MSMs - potentials
  • Often good observational databases exist
  • Should do what we can with them before using
    large amounts of money to do trials
  • Can deal with a time-varying treatment
  • Conceptually fairly straightforward
  • Do not have to model correlation structure in
    responses

41
MSMs - limitations
  • There may always be unmeasured confounders
  • Relies heavily on probability-of-treatment model
    being correct
  • Experimental ETA violations can often occur
    (particularly with small sample sizes)
  • Somewhat inefficient
  • Doubly robust methods may help
  • Not a replacement for an RCT

42
Key points
  • MSMs can help to establish causal associations
    from observational data
  • Make some strong assumptions
  • Need goodness-of-fit for measured confounders
  • Will never find the right model
  • Aim to find good models

43
References
  • Robins JM, Hernan MA, Brumback B. Marginal
    structural models and causal inference in
    epidemiology. Epidemiology 2000 11 550-560.
  • Bang H, Robins JM (2005). Doubly Robust
    Estimation in Missing Data and Causal Inference
    Models. Biometrics 61 (4), 962973.
  • Pullenayegum EM, Lam C, Manlhiot C, Feldman BM.
    Fitting Marginal Structural Models Estimating
    covariate-treatment associations in the
    re-weighted dataset can guide model fitting.
    Journal of Clinical Epidemiology.
  • Wang Y, Petersen ML, Bangsberg D, van der Laan
    MJ. Diagnosing bias in the inverse probability of
    treatment weighted estimator resulting from
    violation of experimental treatment assignment.
    UC Berkeley Division of Biostatistics working
    paper series, 2006.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com