Most Aphasias Are Easily, Although Imprecisely, Diagnosed And Localized - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 34
About This Presentation
Title:

Most Aphasias Are Easily, Although Imprecisely, Diagnosed And Localized

Description:

none – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:185
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 35
Provided by: pamgu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Most Aphasias Are Easily, Although Imprecisely, Diagnosed And Localized


1
Most Aphasias Are Easily, Although Imprecisely,
Diagnosed And Localized
2
  • There are eight principle forms of aphasia, which
    result from cortical lesions.
  • Four of these forms- Brocas aphasia, mixed
    transcortical, transcortical motor and global
    aphasia- have non-fluent spontaneous speech.
  • The remaining four- Wernickes aphasia,
    conduction, transcortical sensory and anomic
    aphasia- have fluent speech.
  • Two-thirds of patients with aphasia can be
    classified into one of these forms. The
    remaining cases are mixed forms or subcortical
    aphasias.

3
  • Aphasia screening requires testing of the
    patients spontaneous speech, comprehension of
    spoken language, repetition of speech,
    confrontational naming ability, reading and
    writing.
  • Language assessment begins by listening to
    spontaneous speech. Two critical questions can
    usually be answered while the patient gives the
    chief complaint
  • Does the patient speak spontaneously?
  • Is speech fluent?

4
  • It is critical to hear the patient speak in
    sentences, so ask open-ended questions such as
    Why have you come to the hospital?, What is
    your typical weekday like? Some patients
    conceal major language disorders with yes and
    no answers.
  • Verbal fluency is the ability to produce flowing,
    smooth speech without word-finding pauses.
    Non-fluent speech is slow, with a rate that is
    usually less than 40 words per minute, sparse, or
    often consisting of single words, effortful,
    dysarthric, dysprosodic and agrammatical.

5
  • Spontaneous speech should also be assessed for
    articulation, prosody and paraphasias. Prosody
    refers to the distribution of stress and melody
    of speech, and includes both affective and
    non-affective components. Prosody is the chief
    affective component of speech and introduces
    subtle shades of meaning or completely changes
    the impact of a statement. Affective prosody is
    the emotional tone that flavors our speech and
    allows our children and pets to understand what
    we are saying even though they cannot comprehend
    words. Non-affective prosody is the alteration in
    pitch and stress that conveys different messages
    with the same words.

6
  • Anterior right hemisphere lesions (analog of
    Brocas area) impair spontaneous prosody, causing
    monotonous speech that lacks emotion and
    intonation, while posterior right hemisphere
    lesions (analog of Wernickes area) impair
    comprehension of prosody, leaving patients unable
    to distinguish subtle shades of meaning in
    others speech.
  • Paraphasias are syllable or word-substitution
    errors and are most common in fluent aphasia.

7
  • Comprehension is assessed after spontaneous
    speech. If the patient constructs
    well-formulated answers to questions,
    comprehension is usually normal.
  • Repetition is tested by asking the patient to
    repeat words, phrases and sentences. There are
    no ifs, ands, or buts about it or The phantom
    soared across the foggy heath. Listen carefully
    for omissions (e.g. plural endings), additions,
    perseverations or repetitions, paraphasias and
    grammatical errors.
  • Confrontational naming is tested by pointing to
    objects and asking the patient to name them.

8
  • Dysarthria, in its strictest sense, refers to
    abnormal articulation. Dysarthria is common in
    anterior non-fluent aphasia, such as Brocas
    aphasia, and rare in posterior fluent aphasia,
    such as Wernickes aphasia.
  • Paretic (flaccid) dysarthria results from
    weakness of articulatory muscles. The voice is
    typically soft or barely audible, low-pitched and
    nasal.

9
  • Spastic (hypertonic, pseudobulbar) dysarthria
    results from upper motor neuron (corticobulbar)
    lesions, usually due to multiple, bilateral small
    lacunar infarcts. Speech is slow, strained,
    thick and monopitched. Articulation is
    imprecise, especially for consonants, and volume
    may be explosive.
  • Ataxic (cerebellar) dysarthria results from
    lesions of the cerebellum or its connections, and
    it is most common in patients with cerebellar
    degeneration and multiple sclerosis. The slurred
    speech is dysarticulated into jerky, irregular
    components. Speech rhythm, volume and pitch are
    variable, and speech may be explosive with poorly
    regulated expiration.

10
  • Palilalia is involuntary repetition of words,
    phrases and less often, syllables. In most
    patients, speech rate increases and volume
    decreases with repetition.
  • Echolalia is the parrot-like repetition of words
    and phrases heard from others.

11
Brocas aphasia consists of non-fluent, effortful
speech with relatively preserved comprehension
following left frontal lesions
12
  • Mutism may occur at the onset of the aphasia, or
    patients may produce only single syllables or
    words. Speech is aspontaneous, sparse and slow
    its rate is fewer than a dozen words per minute.
    Verbal output is agrammatic, even telegraphic,
    because patients utter substantive nouns and
    verbs that carry sentence meaning but omit
    grammatical connecting words, including
    prepositions, articles, conjunctions and adverbs.
    An example is gostoreme, for Do you want to
    go to the store with me?. The writing of
    patients with Brocas aphasia also contains
    grammatical errors.

13
  • The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon, in which
    patients utter the initial letter or syllable of
    a word but cannot complete it, is common in
    Brocas aphasia. Also, confrontational naming is
    usually impaired.
  • Other clinical features of Brocas aphasia
    include severe disruption of articulation and
    prosody (melody and inflection), impairment of
    repetition that is less severe than impairment of
    spontaneous speech, and frequent phonemic
    paraphasias, which consist of sound or syllable
    substitutions.

14
Prosody is the variation in speech rhythm, pitch,
melody, and distribution of stress. Prosody is
the aspect of speech that conveys shades of
meaning through variations in stress and pitch,
independent of words (semantics) and organization
(grammar). Ross and Mesulam (1979) reported two
patients with right frontoparietal strokes who
were unable to express emotional color in their
speech and gestures.
15
Wernickes aphasia is often misdiagnosed as a
psychiatric disorder
16
  • Wernickes aphasia is the antithesis of Brocas
    aphasia. Patients with Wernickes aphasia are
    fluent with normal prosody, often speak
    excessively (logorrhea, verborrhea), create
    paraphasias and neologisms, and have impaired
    comprehension.
  • Fluent aphasia can usually be distinguished from
    psychiatric disorders on historical grounds. The
    behavioral change usually develops suddenly
    without prior personality or mood disorders
    precipitating events, such as death of a spouse,
    are absent and patients are usually old.

17
  • Several features distinguish the speech of
    patients with loose schizophrenia (word
    salad) and Wernickes (or conduction) aphasia.
    Responses to open-ended questions are shorter in
    aphasia paraphasias are common in aphasia and
    rare in schizophrenia vague responses occur in
    both disorders, but are due to word-finding
    problems in aphasia and to circumstantiality in
    psychosis and bizarre and delusional themes
    occur in psychosis.

18
  • Language disorder in Wernickes aphasia is always
    present but varies among patients. Acutely,
    patients may be mute before the characteristic
    fluent, overflowing speech filled with empty
    phrases, circumlocutions and paraphasias.
    Indeed, a foreigner with Wernickes aphasia
    speaking an unknown language would send almost
    identical to one with normal speech. Describing
    an object or event in an indirect fashion
    paraphasias and the lack of meaningful
    substantive words, such as nouns and verbs, are
    most apparent in spontaneous speech.

19
  • Paraphasias are usually verbal rather than
    literal that is, they are more likely to consist
    of word substitutions than substitutions of
    phonemes or sounds. Jargon speech contains
    abundant neologisms, or new words, such as
    thintoke. Children with Wernickes aphasia
    tend to speak less fluently than adults, but
    recover more fully.

20
  • Verbal comprehension deficit is the central
    linguistic component of Wernickes aphasia. The
    best test of comprehension is a series of
    nonsense questions asked in a serious,
    inquisitive manner. Simple questions, such as
    Do two pounds of flour weigh more than one? and
    Are you lying in bed?, can also reveal
    prominent impairment of comprehension.
  • Auditory and reading comprehension are usually
    impaired to a similar degree.

21
  • Patients with Wernickes aphasia also have
    impaired naming and repetition. Writing is
    legible but incomprehensible, with the same
    emptiness, circumlocution and paraphasic errors
    that contaminate speech.
  • Patients with Wernickes aphasia usually recover
    some auditory comprehension, although a favorable
    prognosis is less likely than in Brocas aphasia.

22
Mutism often occurs in non-aphasic disorders
23
  • Initially, patients with Brocas or global
    aphasia may be unable to even attempt speaking.
    Associated symptoms usually include right facial
    weakness or hemisensory loss and hemianopsia in
    global aphasia. As mute aphasic patients
    recover, aspontaneous and non-fluent speech
    emerges. Aphasic patients with mutism make
    linguistic writing errors. These include word
    substitutions or paralexias, such as pair
    instead of chair telegraphic writing in which
    connecting grammatical words are deleted, such
    as, I store rice for I want to go to the store
    to buy rice and non-sensical fluent writing, as
    in, The only thing that I wish well you must
    know that the other thing yes that thing of
    course. These errors help distinguish such
    patients from those with non-aphasic mutism.

24
  • In aphemia muteness often precedes a
    characteristic pattern of slow, hypophonic and
    dysarthric speech
  • Lesions in the supplementary motor area (SMA) of
    the dominant hemisphere can cause muteness during
    the first several weeks. Recovery of spontaneous
    speech and repetition follows, but an impairment
    of writing persists.
  • Abulia may cause mutism. It is a general
    behavioral slowing and lowered activity which
    results from a variety of pathological processes.
    Abulic patients show little spontaneous motor or
    speech activity and may intermittently fail to
    respond to questions or commands for minutes.
    When responses do occur, they are slow and
    apathetic.

25
  • Akinetic mutism represents the most intense form
    of abulia as a condition in which the patient
    appears awake and may follow the examiner with
    his eyes but lacks spontaneous motor and verbal
    responses. In addition, he is doubly incontinent
    and responds incompletely to noxious stimuli.
    Akinetic mutism is also known as locked in
    syndrome

26
  • The locked-in syndrome results from
    de-efferentation of central motor fibers
    supplying facial and body musculature. Lesions
    are located usually in the ventral pons or
    occasionally in the ventral midbrain.
    Pathological processes include infarction,
    hemorrhage, central pontine myelinolysis, tumor
    and encephalitis. Patients are fully alert and
    conscious but can only communicate by blinking or
    moving the eyes. The EEG reveals normal or
    mildly slowed background activity. Any patient
    who is mute should be asked to blink or move the
    eyes on command otherwise, this diagnosis may be
    missed.

27
Locked In Syndrome
  • Mute and paralyzed but for eye movements.
  • Normal cognitive and affective ability
  • Can understand people reading and talking to them
  • Eye blinking in Morse code can be used to
    communicate
  • The cerebral cortex is intact and functioning,
    just disconnected

28
  • Patients with the chronic vegetative state emerge
    from coma and resume relatively normal sleep-wake
    cycles in which the eyes open during
    wakefulness, but intellectual activity is
    minimal or absent. Unlike patients with
    locked-in syndrome, those in the chronic
    vegetative state do not produce any clear-cut,
    high-level response to verbal stimuli. This
    disorder usually follows severe, diffuse brain
    insults such as hypoxia, ischemia, hypoglycemia
    or head trauma. The EEG usually reveals
    background slowing with decreased voltage. The
    prognosis for a meaningful recovery is dismal.

29
Persistent Vegetative State
  • Mute, paralyzed, with spontaneous random eye
    movements and reflexive eye blinks.
  • A decorticate fetal posture is typically held
  • Unaware of surroundings, unresponsive to visitors
    or examiners, some degree of response to very
    basic stimuli such as painful stimuli, strong
    smells, considered devoid of cognitive activity.
  • Show waking and sleeping cycles.

30
Persistent Vegetative State
  • Cerebral cortex and underlying cerebral tissue
    has been destroyed, brain stem structures are
    functioning independently.
  • Vegetative functions such as breathing and
    swallowing operate reflexively.
  • If a person has been in this state for a month or
    more, little chance of recovery but they live for
    years
  • Coma patients may progress to this state

31
Coma
  • A sustained loss of the capacity for arousal,
    preventing any expression of any potential
    cognitive functions.
  • Eyes are closed, sleep wake cycles disappear and
    even extreme stimulation produces no sign of
    response
  • Some coma patients do recover and relate that
    they were in a dream sleep like state from which
    they could not awaken and only limited awareness
    of the external environment. The length of the
    coma is the best predictor of recovery

32
Glasgow Coma Scale
  • Eyes Score
  • Open Spontaneously 4
  • To Verbal Command 3
  • To Pain 2
  • No Response 1

33
GCS
  • Best Motor Response Score
  • To Verbal Command, Obeys 6
  • To Painful Stimulus,
  • Localizes Pain 5
  • Flexion-Withdrawal 4
  • Flexion-Abnormal 3
  • Extension 2
  • No Response 1

34
GCS
  • Best Verbal Response Score
  • Oriented and Converses 5
  • Disoriented and Converses 4
  • Inappropriate words 3
  • Incomprehensible sounds 2
  • No Response 1
  • GCS Total Score 3-15
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com