Discourse Marker Use in People With Aphasia: Familiarity of Listeners - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 17
About This Presentation
Title:

Discourse Marker Use in People With Aphasia: Familiarity of Listeners

Description:

... five min clips from an ABC TV news show about gender differences was used, and were selected. ... Figure 2 shows interjections separately because they were ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:344
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 18
Provided by: convent9
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Discourse Marker Use in People With Aphasia: Familiarity of Listeners


1
 Discourse Marker Use in People With Aphasia
Familiarity of Listeners 
  • Kelly Haylett, B.A.
  • Lisa LaSalle, Ph.D.
  • Communication Sciences Disorders
  • University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire

2
Introduction
  • The Limited Capacity Processor Model (LCP) states
    that a limited amount of cognitive resources are
    available to be allocated to a number of tasks at
    any given time.
  • According the LCP, speech fluency deteriorates
    when multiple demands are placed on the speakers
    cognitive, affective, linguistic, and motor
    skills and abilities.
  • Tasks like speaking to an unfamiliar listener,
    retelling a story unfamiliar to the listener,
    etc., act to increase affective and cognitive
    processing, contributing to deteriorated fluency.
  • Speakers with aphasia are generally disfluent
    speakers, and they produce a wide variety of
    types of disfluencies, errors or paraphasias,
    labeled in a variety of different ways (e.g.,
    verbal self-corrections or revisions).

3
  • Discourse markers (DMs) are speakers (non)verbal
    ways to mark info to the listener
    parenthetically.
  • Some consider DMs separate from disfluencies,
    just as paraphasias are not disfluencies, because
    they are uncorrected. Unlike paraphasias,
    though, sometimes DMs appear helpful to the
    listener.
  • Jucker and Ziv (1996) however, have included
    interjections and revisions as types of DMs.
  • DMs may assist the forward flow of communication
    rather than hinder it. For example, but I
    dont know is a truth-level marker, and may
    function as a way to invite the listener to
    comment if s/he does know.
  • On the other hand, a self-correction or revision
    functions to fix the problem the speaker has
    noted in his/her own speech and so otherwise does
    not add information that might benefit the
    listener.
  • Table 1 shows DM types according to how they
    function.

4
Table 1 Discourse Marker Types(Simmons-Mackie
Damico, 1996, p. 39-41)
  • Initiation
  • Gaining listener attention
  • Termination/Reorientation
  • End of or reorganization of thoughts
  • Participant Role
  • Speaking Turns
  • Affiliation
  • Reinforcing for the listener
  • Truth Level Markers
  • Hedges or Qualifiers

5
  • Speakers typically invite listeners to help them
    convey info, or they qualify the info they
    provide via various types of (non)verbal
    behaviors (e.g., Im not sure).
  • Speakers with aphasia compensate for the fact
    that they no longer have effective verbal ways to
    convey info, and they use what Simmons-Mackie and
    Damico (1996) refer to as a tool box of
    compensatory strategies, DMs being one of those.
  • Much of the research to date on people with
    aphasia has been linguistic in nature, and
    linguistic analysis treats parenthetical remarks
    like artifact.
  • If we knew more about how DMs, errors and
    disfluencies function for speakers with aphasia,
    we could give these usually parenthesed-off
    behaviors their due credit.
  • For a person with aphasia, these behaviors could
    serve as signs of overflow, as in the LCP model,
    or serve as invitations to the clinician to help
    scaffold info, as when discourse markers are
    used.

6
Purpose
  • Therefore, the purpose of this preliminary study
    was to investigate the use of discourse markers,
    disfluencies, and paraphasias in speakers with
    aphasia when they engage in a retell situation
    with a familiar and an unfamiliar listener.

7
Methods Participants
  • Two women (C,S) with mild aphasia were
    recruited on the basis of being similar.
  • Both were five years post-onset.
  • Both scored a 1 on describing task number 12 from
    the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of
    Aphasia (MTDDA 1965).
  • Both scored 5.6 on the Social communication
    subtest of the ASHA Functional Assessment of
    Communication Skills for Adults (FACS 1995). On
    the Communication of basic needs subtest of the
    FACS, Participant C scored a 7.0 Participant S,
    a 6.67.
  • The familiar listener was C and Ss clinician
    the unfamiliar listener was another SLP who had
    not previously met C or S.

8
Procedures
  • Two five min clips from an ABC TV news show about
    gender differences was used, and were selected.
    Clip 1 was played to Participant C and Clip
    2,Participant S.
  • Each participant watched the clip alone so that
    they could retell about it to a listener who had
    not seen it.
  • The familiar and unfamiliar listener conditions
    were counterbalanced.
  • Listeners were instructed to mostly listen, and
    speak as they felt they needed. Naturalness of
    the setting and the situation was emphasized.
    Listeners requested something like, So so tell
    me about what you watched.

9
Coding procedures
  • Participants were audio-videorecorded, verbatim
    transcripts were prepared, and the transcripts
    were coded as follows
  • Discourse markers part of the final message
    considered a mark for the listener (e.g., I
    forget his name).
  • Paraphasias non-words that were uncorrected
  • Interjections um, uh ah (clusters of
    interjections counted as one)
  • Revisions corrected errors, phonological,
    morphological, syntactic, and semantic in type
  • Repetitions sound-syllable, word or
    phrase-level iterations
  • Interrater reliability All codes in the
    transcript were mutually agreed upon between the
    first and second authors. Where disagreements
    occurred, code definitions were further
    elaborated upon.

10
Results
  • As can be seen in Figure 1, the two participants
    produced somewhat more behaviors of all types
    (discourse markers, disfluencies and paraphasias)
    when speaking to the familiar listener.
  • All three figures show that Participant S
    produced more interjections and other types of
    disfluencies. However, both participants data
    can be categorized in the following way

11
(No Transcript)
12
(No Transcript)
13
disfluencies
14
  • Figure 2 shows interjections separately because
    they were the most prevalent disfluency type,
    produced on 7-25 of sample words.
  • Figure 3 shows that revisions and repetitions
    were the next most frequently occurring
    disfluency types there is a trend for higher
    occurrence in the familiar listener condition.
  • Whereas discourse markers tend to occur more
    frequently in the Unfamiliar listener condition,
    and paraphasias are questionable due to
    inter-subject variability.
  • Discourse markers tended to be of the truth-level
    type (4/6 Participant C 5/6 Participant S)
    (e.g., or something like that). Initiation
    (e.g., First of all) or participant role (e.g.,
    so..) functions were identified for the other
    3/12 (25) DMs.

11
15
Discussion
  • Little or no support for the idea of cognitive
    overflow was found in this preliminary study.
    Support would have come from finding more
    disfluencies in the Unfamiliar listener condition
    than in the Familiar.
  • Perhaps Jucker Zin (1996) and others are
    correct that interjections and revisions function
    like discourse markers, as a way to mark for the
    listener info to the effect of hold on, Im
    formulating. Perhaps the speaker is freed-up to
    use more interjections and revisions when
    speaking to a familiar listener because she is
    more expressive due to lower demands in this
    condition.
  • On the other hand, discourse markers may occur
    more frequently in the Unfamiliar listener
    condition because the speaker with aphasia feels
    an obligation to a stranger to get it right,
    that is to communicate qualifiers and mostly
    truth-level information.

12
16
  • Future investigations into nonverbal behaviors
    and consistency of discourse marker use from more
    samples should prove useful to those who are
    interested in the idea that People with aphasia
    communicate better than they talk (Holland,
    1982).
  • Simmons-Mackie and Damico (2002) provide the
    potential use of base layer and gaze/gesture
    layer of analysis, for example.
  • It should be noted that we did not ask listeners
    to do any work toward supporting the speakers.
    Future research should investigate strategies
    such as listener scaffolding in a retell (e.g.,
    first episode is restated before the next is
    retold), and use of hierarchical models (e.g.,
    first the familiar than the unfamiliar listener
    is introduced to the client or patient), and the
    effect of these strategies on the fluency of
    speakers with aphasia.  

13
17
References
  • Holland, A. (1982). Observing functional
    communication in aphasic adults. Journal of
    Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 50-56.
  • Jucker, A. H., Ziv, Y. (1998). Discourse
    markers descriptions and theory. Philadelphia J.
    Benjamins.
  • Schuell, H. (1965). Minnesota Test for
    Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia. Minneapolis,
    MN. University of Minnesota Press.
  • Simmons-Mackie, N. N., Damico, J. S. (2002).The
    base layer and the gaze/gesture layer of
  • transcription. Clinical Linguistics and
    Phonetics, 16, 317-327.
  • Simmons-Mackie, N. N., Damico, J. S. (1996).
    The contribution of discourse markers to
    communicative competence in aphasia. American
    Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5, 37-43.

14
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com