Phonological Impairment - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Phonological Impairment

Description:

... second author in randomized order over three individual testing ... CTOPP Rapid Naming mean composite standard score Group 1 45th percentile average ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:395
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 21
Provided by: monicagord1
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Phonological Impairment


1
Phonological Impairments Relation to
Phonological Awareness, Working Memory, and
Literacy Monica Gordon Pershey, Ed.D.,
CCC-SLP Department of Speech and
Hearing Cleveland State University Cleveland,
OH mgpershey_at_att.net Patricia A. Clickner, M.A.,
CCC-SLP Lorain County Board of Mental Retardation
and Developmental Disabilities Elyria,
OH pclickner_at_loraincountymrdd.org
2
Background
  • Developmental phonological impairment may arise
    from inadequate mental
  • representations of speech sounds and/or
    difficulty accessing representations of speech
    sounds.
  • Possibly 35 of children with phonological
    impairment evidence subsequent reading and
    spelling difficulties.
  • Some children with phonological impairment may
    have difficulty acquiring phonological
  • awareness, i.e., the metalinguistic ability to
    reflect upon and manipulate speech sounds.
  • Phonological awareness bootstraps onto the
    childs speech system, with instabilities in the
    speech system potentially constraining
    phonological awareness. Strong and accurate
    internal phonological representations meaning
    accurate speech sound productions provoke the
    association of phoneme to grapheme, allowing
    reading and spelling to develop.
  • The question remains whether it is weakness in
    phonological awareness alone that places children
    with phonological impairment at risk for reading
    and spelling difficulties. There is ample
    research evidence that a double deficit in two
    core processing deficits underlie reading
    disability (1) undeveloped phonological
    awareness and (2) inadequate rapid naming of
    visual symbols. For children with phonological
    impairment, a proposed double deficit would
    involve (1) a weakness in internally representing
    and/or accessing speech sounds that impacts upon
    phonological awareness and that coexists with (2)
    difficulty rapidly retrieving names for visual
    symbols.

3
Background
  • A Proposed Double Deficit for Children with
    Phonological Impairment
  • Some children with phonological impairment may
    have comorbid deficits in verbal working memory
    that manifest as difficulties with short term
    storage and manipulation of information that is
    encountered as auditory and/or verbal input.
  • Verbal working memory span allows phonological
    and/or orthographic information to be On Screen
    for a long enough period of time for
    manipulations to be performed. Rapid naming of
    colors, letters, or numbers demonstrates verbal
    working memory.
  • Rapid naming of letters reveals additional,
    separate cognitive-linguistic processes critical
    for learning to read. Each time a letter is
    encountered, it must be matched to letter
    templates or prototypes stored in visual memory
    and then matched to its name. Trouble rapidly
    naming letters shows deficiencies related to (a)
    orthographic representations (i.e., difficulties
    with visual symbols that enter memory as icons
    which are then arbitrarily named and stored as
    semantic memory), and/or (b) adequate storage of
    visual information but inadequate semantic labels
    for the names of letters, and/or (c) interference
    from inadequate phonological representations,
    with instabilities in the speech system
    constraining knowledge of letter names and/or
    sounds. For children with phonological
    impairment, we must also rule out diminished
    naming speed that can be attributed to reduced
    articulatory proficiency.

4
Purpose
  • The purpose of this study was to explore
  • whether children with phonological impairment
  • would show associated deficits in speech
  • motor control, phonological awareness, verbal
  • working memory, reading, and spelling.
  • This research explored whether deficits in
  • phonological awareness and rapid naming
  • converged in a sample of children with
  • phonological impairment and determined the
    combined
  • impact of this double deficit on reading and
    spelling.

5
Research Questions
  • Research Questions
  • Group Comparisons
  • Do children with phonological impairment evidence
    lower scores on tests of physiologically-based
    functions, namely suprasegmental quality of
    speech and oral motor control, than typically
    developing peers?
  • Do children with phonological impairment evidence
    lower scores on cognitive-linguistic tasks that
    tax verbal working memory than typically
    developing peers?
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on rapid naming testing than
    typically developing peers?
  • Do children with phonological impairment evidence
    lower scores on phonological awareness testing
    than typically developing peers?
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on reading testing than typically
    developing peers?
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on spelling testing than typically
    developing peers?

6
Research Questions
  • Research Questions
  • Measures of Association Among Variables
  • Are deficits in physiologically-based functions,
    namely suprasegmental quality of speech and oral
    motor control, related to the presence of
    phonological impairment?
  • Are deficits in cognitive-linguistic tasks that
    tax verbal working memory related to the presence
    of phonological impairment?
  • Are deficits in rapid naming associated with
    phonological impairment?
  • Are deficits in phonological awareness associated
    with phonological impairment?
  • Are deficits in reading associated with
    phonological impairment?
  • Are deficits in spelling associated with
    phonological impairment?

7
Methodology
  • Participants
  • Group 1 - 23 English-speaking children (12 in 1st
    grade, 8 in 2nd grade, 3 in 3rd grade) diagnosed
    with phonological impairment
  • Group 2 - 23 phonologically unimpaired peers (12
    in 1st grade, 8 in 2nd grade, 3 in 3rd grade)
    matched for race, gender, age (range 6.4 - 9.1),
    grade level, free lunch status, and IQ (normal
    range)
  • From 10 elementary schools in one Midwest county,
    median household income 60,000
  • All Group 1 and 2 students had passing scores for
    hearing acuity Pure-tone air-conduction hearing
    screening at 20 db HL for 500, 1000, 2000, and
    4000 Hz
  • Groups 1 and 2 are significantly different due to
    level of phonological impairment as assessed by
    the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2
    (GFTA-2) (with findings used to compute
    Percentage of Consonants Correct) ANOVA F(1,
    44) 55.16, p lt.0001

8
Methodology
  • Methodology
  • Three areas of capabilities were assessed for all
    participants.
  • Administration of tests was conducted by the
    second author in randomized order over three
    individual testing sessions per child.
  • Mean scores on all measures for groups 1 and 2
    were computed.
  • Area 1 - Physiological factors
  • To assess suprasegmental qualities of speech A
    brief conversational sample analyzed for each
    participant for presence/absence of adequate
    quality scored as 0" for normal, 1" for
    deviated 10 or less of the time, or 2" for
    deviated greater than 10 of the time
  • To assess oral motor skills Zelvis Oral
    Peripheral Screening - scored as 1" for normal
    diadochokinetic rate or 2" for abnormally slowed
    diadochokinetic rate for each participant

9
Methodology
  • Area 2 - Cognitive-linguistic factors
  • To assess verbal working memory The Clinical
    Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-3 (CELF-3)
    subtests for word forms (sentence recall allows
    child to generate needed syntax/grammar/morphology
    ), following directions, sentence repetition
    Standard scores obtained for each participant
  • To assess rapid naming The Comprehensive Test
    of Phonological Processes (CTOPP) Rapid Naming
    Subtests - colors, digits, letters, and
    non-words Standard scores obtained for each
    participant
  • To assess phonological awareness CTOPP
    (Composite of Elision, Blending, Sound Matching
    subtests) Standard scores obtained for each
    participant

10
Methodology
  • Area 3 - Reading and Spelling
  • Reading and Spelling subtests from The Kaufman
    Test of Educational Achievement (K-TEA) brief
    form Standard scores obtained for each
    participant

11
Results
  • Group Comparisons
  • Area 1
  • Do children with phonological impairment evidence
    lower scores on tests of physiologically-based
    functions, namely suprasegmental quality of
    speech and oral motor control, than typically
    developing peers?
  • Suprasegmental Quality
  • The difference between groups is
    significant ANOVA F(1, 44) 7.48, p .009
  • Oral Motor Control
  • 22 of Group 1 children had abnormal
    diadochokinetic performance
  • The difference between groups is
    significant ANOVA F(1, 44) 6.11, p .017

12
Results
  • Group Comparisons
  • Area 2
  • Do children with phonological impairment evidence
    lower scores on cognitive-linguistic tasks that
    tax verbal working memory than typically
    developing peers?
  • CELF-3 Word Forms
  • The difference between groups is not significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 3.67, p .062
  • CELF-3 Direction Following
  • The difference between groups is not significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 3.04, p .088
  • CELF-3 Sentence Repetition
  • The difference between groups is significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 5.55, p .023
  • CTOPP Non-word Repetition
  • The difference between groups is significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 10.39, p .002
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on rapid naming testing than
    typically developing peers?
  • CTOPP Rapid Naming mean composite standard score
    Group 1 45th percentile average performance
    range

13
Results
  • Area 3
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on reading testing than typically
    developing peers?
  • K-TEA Reading standard score Group 1 34th
    percentile lt 1 SD below normative mean
  • Group 2 74th percentile lt 1 SD below normative
    mean
  • The difference between groups is significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 31.72, p lt.0001
  • Do children with phonological impairment perform
    more poorly on spelling testing than typically
    developing peers?
  • K-TEA Spelling standard score Group 1 37th
    percentile lt 1 SD below normative mean
  • Group 2 68th percentile lt 1 SD below normative
    mean
  • The difference between groups is significant
    ANOVA F(1, 44) 9.84, p .003
  • Just to be sure - How different are these two
    groups?
  • A Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA)
    compared groups based on all variables combined -
    speech physiology, phonological awareness, verbal
    working memory, rapid naming, reading, spelling
  • The difference between groups is significant
  • MANOVA F(1, 44) 14.25, p lt.0001
  • MANOVA compared groups based on two variables
    combined - reading and spelling
  • The difference between groups is significant
  • MANOVA F(1, 44) 15.58, p lt.0001

14
Results
  • Measures of Association Among Variables for
    Children with Phonological Impairment
  • Phonological impairment was associated with
    deficits in oral motor control and working
    memory, including rapid naming, but not
    phonological awareness, reading, or spelling
  • Pearson Product Moment Correlations of Mean
    Scores - Percentage of Consonants Correct
    correlated with Other Variables
  • Are deficits in physiologically-based functions,
    namely suprasegmental quality of speech and oral
    motor control, related to the presence of
    phonological impairment?
  • Suprasegmental Quality - No Correlation
  • Oral Motor Control - Significant Correlation r
    -.597, p .003
  • Lower Percentage of Consonants Correct scores
    were associated with higher (poorer)
    diadochokinetic scores, creating an inverse
    relationship
  • Are deficits in cognitive-linguistic tasks that
    tax verbal working memory related to the presence
    of phonological impairment?
  • CELF-3 Word Forms - Significant Correlation r
    .517, p .012
  • CELF-3 Direction Following Significant
    Correlation r .612, p .002
  • CELF-3 Sentence Repetition Significant
    Correlation r .464, p .026
  • CTOPP Non-word Repetition - No Correlation

15
Results
  • Are deficits in rapid naming associated with
    phonological impairment?
  • CTOPP Rapid Naming Composite - Significant
    Correlation
  • r .405, p .05
  • Are deficits in phonological awareness associated
    with phonological impairment?
  • CTOPP Phonological Awareness Composite - No
    correlation
  • Are deficits in reading associated with
    phonological impairment?
  • K-TEA Reading - No correlation
  • Are deficits in spelling associated with
    phonological impairment?
  • K-TEA Spelling - No correlation

16
Results
  • Stepwise Regression Data
  • Correlational data did not fully describe whether
    phonological impairment was associated with
    deficits in rapid naming
  • (at p .05), phonological awareness, reading,
    and/or spelling
  • Which test scores for children with phonological
    impairment accounted for variance in reading and
    spelling test scores?
  • 41 of the variance in K-TEA Reading scores could
    be accounted for by performance on CTOPP
    Phonological Awareness Composite (p lt.001)
  • 69 of the variance in K-TEA Reading scores
    could be accounted for by performance on CTOPP
    Phonological Awareness Composite and CTOPP Rapid
    Naming Composite scores as a combined independent
    variable (p lt.0001) Coefficient analysis PA at
    p lt.0001 RN at p .004 - indicates adequate
    stringency for each predictor variable
  • 64 of the variance in K-TEA Spelling scores
    could be accounted for by performance on CTOPP
    Phonological Awareness Composite (p .008)
  • 49 of the variance in K-TEA Spelling scores
    could be accounted for by performance on CTOPP
    Rapid Naming Composite (p .012)
  • Need to finally reduce to explained/unexplained
    variance
  • 66 of the variance in K-TEA Reading and Spelling
    scores as a combined dependent variable could be
    accounted for by performance on CTOPP
    Phonological Awareness Composite and CTOPP Rapid
    Naming Composite scores as a combined independent
    variable (p lt.0001) Coefficient analysis PA at
    p lt.0001 RN at p .009 - indicates adequate
    stringency for each predictor variable

17
Conclusions
  • The difference between children with phonological
    impairment and matched unimpaired children was
    significant for all measures except two verbal
    working memory tasks (not rapid naming).
  • The presence of speech motor deficits was not
    ruled out. Phonological impairment correlated
    with poorer performance on speech motor tasks.
  • Children with phonological impairment performed
    below the normative mean on tests of rapid
    naming, phonological awareness, reading, and
    spelling. Phonological impairment correlated with
    poorer performance on cognitive-linguistic tasks
    that tax verbal working memory and on rapid
    naming.
  • Depressed rapid naming and phonological awareness
    coexisted as a double deficit in this sample of
    children with phonological impairment.
  • Findings support prior research that children
    with reading deficits may have difficulties with
    verbal working memory.
  • Findings support prior reports that children with
    phonological impairments may manifest deficits in
    verbal working memory, specifically rapid naming.

18
Conclusions
  • Practical Applications
  • Speech disturbances which render a child
    difficult to understand should not be dismissed
    as developmental motor skill.
  • For children with phonological impairment who
    have weak phonological awareness and rapid
    naming, the double deficit hypothesis is
    applicable internal representations of speech
    sounds must be continually monitored and
    reinforced.
  • All children with phonological impairment should
    be assessed by a speech-language pathologist and
    a reading specialist for deficits in rapid
    naming, phonological awareness, reading, and
    spelling.
  • Intensive speech, language, and literacy
    interventions should be applied for children with
    coexisting phonological impairment and verbal
    memory deficits.
  • Future Research
  • Can research distinguish when deficient rapid
    naming ability reflects difficulties with verbal
    memory AND/OR difficulties with speech motor
    control? Studies need to separate these
    performance components to isolate the memory vs
    motor aspects of rapid naming.
  • Can research establish subtypes of phonological
    impairment, as in with verbal memory impairment,
    with speech motor impairment, or with both?
  • Research is needed to explore any and all
    connections between phonological impairment and
    literacy acquisition.

19
References
  • Bird, J., Bishop, D. V. M., Freeman, N. H.
    (1995). Phonological awareness and literacy
    development in children with expressive
    phonological impairments. Journal of Speech and
    Hearing Research, 38, 446-462.
  • Bishop, D. M. V., Adams, C. (1990). A
    prospective study of the relationship between
    specific language impairment, phonological
    disorders and reading retardation. Journal of
    Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027-1050.
  • Catts, H. W. (1991). Phonological processing
    deficits and reading disabilities. In A. G. Kahmi
    H. W. Catts (Eds.), Reading disabilities, A
    developmental language perspective (pp. 67-99).
    Needham Heights, MA Allyn Bacon. Lewis, B. A.,
    Freebairn, L., Taylor, H. G. (2000). Academic
    outcomes in children with histories of speech
    sound disorders. Journal of Communication
    Disorders, 33, 11-30.
  • Lewis, B. A., Freebairn, L., Taylor, H. G.
    (2002). Correlates of spelling abilities in
    children with speech sound disorders. Reading
    Writing An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15,
    389-407.
  • Lovett, M., W., Steinbach, K.A., Frijters, J.C.
    (2000). Remediating the core deficits of
    developmental reading disability A double
    deficit perspective. Journal of Learning
    Disabilities, 33, 334-359.
  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M.
    (1999). See Dick RAN Rapid naming and the
    longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in
    first and second graders. Scientific Studies of
    Reading. 3, 129-157.
  • National Institute on Deafness and Other
    Communication Disorders (NIDCD). (2000).
    Incidence and prevalence of speech, voice, and
    language disorders in the United States.
    Bethesda, MD NICD. Retrieved October 5, 2002,
    from http//www.nih,gov/nidcd/health/vsl.htm
  • Schatschneider, C., Calrson, C. D., Francis, D.
    J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M. (2002).
    Relationship of rapid automatized naming and
    phonological awareness in early reading
    development implications for the double-deficit
    hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 35,
    245-256. Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y.
    (1992). Phonology and reading disability. Ann
    Arbor, MI The University of Michigan Press.
  • Shriberg, L., Kwiatkowski, J., Best, S., Hengst,
    J., Terselic-Weber, B. (1986). Characteristics
    of children with phonologic disorders of unknown
    origin. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
    51, 140-161.
  • Shriberg, L., Kwiatkowski, J. (1994).
    Developmental phonological disorders I A
    clinical profile. Journal of Speech and Hearing
    Research, 37, 1100-1126.
  • Stackhouse, J., Wells, B. (2001). Childrens
    speech and literacy difficulties A
    psycholinguistic framework. London, England
    Whurr Publishers Ltd.
  • Torgesen, J. K. (1999). Assessment and
    instruction for phonetic awareness and word
    recognition skills. In Kamhi, A. Catts, H.
    (Eds.), Reading dsabilities, A developmental
    language perspective. (pp. 128-153). Needham
    Heights, MA Allyn Bacon.
  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). American factfinder.
    Washington, D.C. Author. Retrieved September 20,
    2002, from http//factfinder.census.gov
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A.
    (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological
    Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX Pro-Ed.
  • Webster, P. E., Plante, A. S. (1992). Effects
    of phonological impairment on word, syllable, and
    phoneme segmentation and reading. Language,
    Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 23,
    176-182.
  • Webster, P. E., Plante, A. S. (1995).
    Productive phonology and phonological awareness
    in preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics,
    16, 43-57.
  • Webster, P. E., Plante, A. S., Couvillion, L.
    M. (1997). Phonologic impairment and prereading
    Update on a longitudinal study. Journal of
    Leaning Disabilities, 30, 365-375.
  • Wolf, M., Goldberg ORourke, A., Gidney, C.,
    Lovett, M., Cirino, P., Morris, R. (2002). The
    second deficit An investigation of the
    independence of phonological and naming-speed
    deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and
    Writing An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15,
    43-72.

20
(No Transcript)
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com