Contrast and the realization of schwa vowels in English - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Contrast and the realization of schwa vowels in English

Description:

Contrast and the realization of schwa vowels in English Edward Flemming Introduction The project: To derive properties of phonetic and phonological vowel reduction ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:355
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 56
Provided by: Tria308
Learn more at: http://web.mit.edu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Contrast and the realization of schwa vowels in English


1
Contrast and the realization of schwa vowels in
English
  • Edward Flemming

2
Introduction
  • The project
  • To derive properties of phonetic and phonological
    vowel reduction from general constraints related
    to speech production and perception
  • Outline of the talk
  • Outline a model of vowel reduction.
  • Explore its application to English vowel
    reduction (reduction to schwa).

3
Phonological vowel reduction
  • Vowel contrasts are neutralized in unstressed
    syllables.
  • E.g. Southern Italian (Mistretta dialect, Mazzola
    1976)
  • Primary stressed Elsewhere
  • i u i u
  • e o a
  • a

stressed vowels unstressed vowels i vin?i he
sells vin?imu we sell e ve_at_ni he
comes (vinimu we come) a a_at_vi he
has aviti he has o mo_at_ri he
dies (murimu we die) u u_at_Ô?i he
boils uÔ?imu we boil
4
Phonological vowel reduction
  • Common patterns of vowel reduction
  • (a) i u (b) i u (c) i u
  • e o e o a/
  • E ç a
  • a
  • (a) reduces to (b), e.g. Standard Italian, B.
    Portuguese, Slovene
  • (b) reduces to (c), e.g. Standard Russian, S.
    Italian, Catalan
  • (a) reduces to (c), e.g. E. Catalan
  • Reduction to a single vowel, e.g. English, Dutch,
    Salish
  • Primarily neutralization of height contrasts.

5
Outline of an analysis of vowel reduction
  • Vowel reduction is fundamentally motivated by
    undershoot in short unstressed syllables.

6
Phonetic vowel reduction - Undershoot
7
Outline of an analysis of vowel reduction
  • Vowel reduction is fundamentally motivated by
    undershoot in short unstressed syllables.
  • Short duration of unstressed vowels increases the
    effort required to achieve distinct vowel
    qualities, particularly low vowels (Lindblom
    1963).
  • Contrasts are subject to distinctiveness
    constraints, so neutralization occurs where
    phonetic reduction would otherwise render
    contrasts insufficiently distinct.

8
Undershoot as a consequence of effort
minimization
  • Faster movements are more effortful (Nelson 1983,
    Perkell et al 2002).
  • In a CVC sequence, the articulators have to move
    to and from the position for the vowel.
  • Undershoot results from avoiding fast movements.

9
Formant undershoot as a function of duration and
distance
Lindbloms model F2V k2(F2i F2t)e-?TF2t if
F1t ? 375 Hz F1V F1t if F1t gt 375 Hz
F1V k1(375 F1t)e-?TF1t F2i is F2 at C
release F1t, F2t are V formant targets k, ?
depend on consonant context
  • More undershoot where
  • Vowel is shorter
  • Distance between C and V is greater

10
Formant undershoot as a function of duration and
distance
Lindbloms model F2V k2(F2i F2t)e-?TF2t if
F1t ? 375 Hz F1V F1t if F1t gt 375 Hz
F1V k1(375 F1t)e-?TF1t F2i is F2 at C
release F1t, F2t are V formant targets k, ?
depend on consonant context
  • More undershoot where
  • Vowel is shorter
  • Distance between C and V is greater

11
Implementation of the model of vowel reduction
  • Stressed and unstressed inventories of
    contrasting vowel categories are selected from a
    space of possible vowels so as to best satisfy
    constraints on contrasts
  • Maximize distinctiveness of contrasts.
  • Maximize number of contrasts.
  • Minimize articulatory effort.
  • Effort minimization implies undershoot.

12
Model of vowel reduction
  • The vowel space is modeled on Liljencrants and
    Lindblom (1972).

13
i. Maximize the distinctiveness of contrasts
  • Distinctiveness of the contrast between Vi and Vj
    is the (weighted) distance between the vowels in
    formant space.

Where xn is F2 of Vn in Bark yn is F1 of Vn in
Bark a lt 1
14
i. Maximize the distinctiveness of contrasts
  • Overall distinctiveness cost of a vowel system
    depends on the minimum distance found in either
    inventory.

where
Cost
15
ii. Maximize the number of contrasts
  • maximize the number of vowels in the stressed and
    unstressed vowel inventories.

where
Cost
16
iii. Articulatory effort
  • The space of possible vowels contracts as vowel
    duration is reduced, following the undershoot
    functions proposed by Lindblom (1963)
  • Consonants are assumed to assimilate partially to
    the vowel target in F2, but not in F1.

17
Overall cost function
  • The optimal vowel system is the one that best
    satisfies these constraints

(subject to vowel space constraint)
18
Optimal inventories
a 0.14, k1 1.5, ?1 0.008, k2 1.5, ?1
0.01, c 0.27, F2l 1400 Hz, wn
6 Durations stressed 160 ms Unstressed 100 ms
19
Italian vowels
Data from Albano Leoni et al 1995
20
Undershoot and vowel reduction
  • Relating phonological vowel reduction to
    undershoot helps to explain
  • The tendency to neutralize vowel contrasts in
    short unstressed syllables.
  • The generalization that vowel reduction primarily
    eliminates height contrasts.
  • The generalization that neutralizing vowel
    reduction is accompanied by phonetic reduction.

21
English reduction to schwa
  • English exhibits a pattern of vowel reduction
    whereby vowel quality contrasts are neutralized
    in unstressed syllables.
  • The resulting vowel is usually transcribed as
    schwa
  • atom QRm
  • atomic tHAmIk

22
Reduction to schwa
  • Predictions of the undershoot model
  • Reduction to a single vowel should be most likely
    where vowels are very short.
  • Where there is a single vowel, distinctiveness of
    vowel quality contrasts is irrelevant, so effort
    minimization should dominate.
  • So schwa should be a transitional vowel,
    maximally assimilated to the surrounding context.
  • targetless schwa

23
Minimum effort vowels
  • Minimal deviation from the narrow constrictions
    for surrounding consonants results in low F1 (a
    high vowel) because any constriction above the
    pharynx lowers F1.
  • Minimal deviation from the tongue body and lip
    positions for surrounding consonants and vowels
    results in contextually variable F2.
  • But schwa is often said to be a mid central
    vowel.

24
Experiment 1 English schwa vowels( research
with Stephanie Johnson)
  • Materials

final non-final
Rosa rhapsody
Lisa suggest
Russia suspend
asia prejudice
ninja today
sofa begin
vodka report
soda compare
alpha umbrella probable
alpha umbrella suffocate
25
Experiment 1
  • Also recorded full vowels for comparison.
  • heed i, hid I, head E, had Q, odd A,
  • hood U, who u
  • Spoken in carrier phrase Say ___ to me.
  • 9 female speakers of American English.
  • Measured first two formants at the mid point of
    the vowels.
  • compare frequently lacked any voiced vowel in the
    first syllable, so it was excluded from analysis.

26
Results
  • Non-final schwa
  • Low F1 (mean 425 Hz)
  • F2 is contextually variable.

27
Results
  • Non-final schwa
  • Low F1 (mean 425 Hz)
  • F2 is contextually variable.
  • Final schwa
  • F1 shows wide range (mean 665 Hz).
  • Much of this is between-speaker variation.
  • Central F2 (mean 1772 Hz)

28
Results
  • Final schwa
  • F1 shows wide range (mean 665 Hz).
  • Much of this is between-speaker variation.
  • vocal tract size
  • quality of final schwa ( - v)

29
Two patterns of vowel reduction
  • The difference between final and non-final schwa
    vowels can interpreted in terms of the undershoot
    model of vowel reduction.
  • There are two degrees of unstressed vowel
    reduction, depending on characteristic vowel
    duration.

30
Two patterns of vowel reduction
  • Final unstressed vowels are longer than non-final
    unstressed vowels 153 ms vs. 64 ms.
  • Presumably a result of final lengthening.
  • Allows for a lower schwa vowel, which in turns
    allows for the maintenance of contrasts
  • The vowels i, , oU and rhotic contrast in
    unstressed syllables contrast in absolute
    word-final position.
  • Non-final schwa does not minimally contrast with
    other vowel qualities.
  • Consequently it is assimilated to context.

31
The correlation between contrast and reduced
vowel quality
  • The same correlation between system of contrasts
    and reduced vowel quality is observed across
    languages
  • Contextually variable high vowels only seems to
    be found where all vowel quality contrasts are
    neutralized, e.g. Dutch (van Bergem 1994),
    probably Montana Salish.
  • Mid central is found in contrast with higher
    vowels, e.g. reduced vowel inventories of the
    form i, , u occur in unstressed syllables in
    Russian (Padgett and Tabain 2003), E. Bulgarian
    (Wood and Pettersson 1988), Catalan (Herrick
    2003), and in final unstressed syllables in
    Brazilian Portuguese (Mattoso Camara 1972).

32
The correlation between contrast and reduced
vowel quality
  • Central Catalan stressed and unstressed vowels
    (Herrick 2003).

i
u
e
o

E
ç
a
33
Experiment 2 Targetless schwa
  • This analysis hypothesizes that the non-final
    reduced vowel in English is a minimum effort
    vowel.
  • So far based on impressionistic evaluation of a
    relatively unsystematic sample of medial schwa
    vowels.

34
Experiment 2 Targetless schwa
  • Systematically vary the preceding and following
    context of medial schwa.
  • Questions
  • Does variability of schwa involve assimilation to
    the surrounding context?
  • Is there any evidence that schwa has a vowel
    quality target?
  • Conclusions
  • Much of the variability of schwa can be
    attributed to assimilation.
  • Schwa lacks a vowel quality target, but it is not
    completely targetless - its target is to be a
    vowel.

35
Materials
  • Nonce words of the form bV1C1C2V2t
  • All combinations of V1 from i, œ, u
  • Cn from b, d, g
  • V2 from i, A, u
  • (81 words)
  • Subjects were instructed to model the stress
    pattern on words like propagate and parakeet.
  • e.g. bidgut, bQgbit, etc.

36
Materials
  • All words were read in the sentence frame
  • X. Do you know what a X is?
  • Presented twice in random order (only second run
    is analyzed here).
  • Read by four native speakers of American English,
    2 male, 2 female.

37
Analysis
  • Measured F1 and F2
  • at steady state, extreme values, or midpoint of
    V1 and V2
  • at the offset of V1, and at the onset of voicing
    in V2.
  • in schwa at the onset of voicing, at the offset
    of schwa, and half way between these points.

38
Results
formants at midpoint of schwa
39
Results
  • Medial schwa is highly variable in quality,
    particularly in F2.

40
Is this variability the result of interpolation
between preceding and following context?
  • F2 in schwa is correlated with F2 of surrounding
    vowels
  • But it is not the result of simple interpolation
    between vowels.

41
Is schwa variability the result of interpolation?
  • Unsurprisingly, the consonants also have a
    significant influence on F2 of schwa.

42
Is schwa variability the result of interpolation?
  • The F2 trajectory of schwa is more likely to be
    an interpolation between preceding and following
    consonants.
  • But F2 adjacent to a consonant depends in turn on
    F2 of the adjacent vowel.

43
Locus equations
  • Typically consonant F2 is a linear function of F2
    at the midpoint of the adjacent vowel (Lindblom
    1963, Klatt 1987, etc).
  • The slope and intercept of this function depend
    on the consonant.

bçd
bid
44
Is schwa variability the result of interpolation?
  • These considerations suggest the following model
    of F2 at schwa midpoint
  • F2Smid aC1F2V1 bC1 cC2F2V2 dC2
  • The effect of the vowels on F2 of schwa is
    modulated by the intervening consonants.
  • This model is quite successful (r2 0.73-0.86
    for individual subjects)
  • For comparison, a model based on F2 at the vowel
    mid points alone yields r2 of 0.19-0.36

45
Summary for F2
  • Medial schwas show wide variation in F2.
  • This variation is systematically conditioned by
    context.
  • It is difficult to determine whether schwa F2 is
    the result of simple interpolation.

46
Is F1 variability the result of interpolation?
  • F1 at schwa midpoint also varies substantially
    according to vowel context, but cannot be the
    result of interpolation between preceding and
    following vowels.

47
Is F1 variability the result of interpolation?
  • The consonants can account for the fact that
    schwa F1 tends to be much lower than in adjacent
    vowels forming a stop closure lowers F1.
  • But if F1 in schwa is governed by the adjacent
    consonantal constrictions, then we would expect
    schwa vowels to have very low F1.

48
Is F1 variability the result of interpolation?
  • In iCCi, F1 at schwa midpoint is higher than in
    preceding or following vowels.
  • Schwa appears to have an F1 (height) target.
  • If there are no height contrasts, why is there a
    height target?

49
Height target or presence target?
  • While non-final schwa does not contrast with
    other vowels in quality, it is still important to
    distinguish presence vs. absence of schwa.
  • In some contexts presence vs. absence of schwa is
    minimally contrastive.
  • about baUt vs. bout baUt
  • parade pHeId prayed pHeId
  • support spHçt sport spçt
  • So schwa is expected to have targets related to
    signaling the presence of a vowel, e.g.
  • duration
  • intensity peak

50
Height target or presence target?
  • Producing an intensity peak generally involves
    increasing F1 (cf. Howitt 2000).
  • So the apparent F1 target for non-final schwa may
    be a byproduct of signaling the presence of a
    vowel.
  • This interpretation predicts that the target
    for F1 is not a specific value, but a minimum
    value. Accordingly, schwa should assimilate to
    its context where this would result in high F1 -
    e.g. adjacent to a low vowel.

51
  • Preliminary evidence suggests that schwa fully
    assimilates to the F1 of an adjacent low vowel.
    E.g. Mrs Shah adressed the audience
  • If schwa had a specific F1 target (e.g. 400 Hz),
    then we should observe movement towards this
    target.

Mrs Shah addressed...
Mr Shah dressed...
52
Summary of experiment 2
  • The quality of medial schwa vowels is highly
    variable.
  • Variation in F2 is particularly extensive, and
    involves assimilation to the surrounding context,
    suggesting that medial schwa has no specific
    backness or rounding targets.
  • F1 in schwa also covaries with F1 of surrounding
    vowels, but systematically deviates from F1 of
    the context, suggesting that schwa has an F1
    target.
  • This apparent F1 target may be a byproduct of
    realizing an intensity peak to cue the presence
    of a vowel.
  • it is important to consider contrasts with
    absence as well as quality contrasts as sources
    of constraint on the realization of segments.

53
Conclusion
  • English shows two patterns of vowel reduction,
    both are consistent with the undershoot model of
    vowel reduction.
  • Non-final schwa vowels are short, which would
    tend to lead to substantial undershoot.
  • Vowel quality contrasts are neutralized.
  • The resulting vowel is substantially assimilated
    to its context.
  • But assimilation is limited by the need to
    provide clear cues to the presence of a vowel,
    e.g. an intensity peak.
  • Final unstressed position has longer vowels,
    allowing for vowel quality contrasts.
  • Schwa is realized as a mid central vowel,
    distinct from /i, oU/.
  • The same correlation between system of contrasts
    and the quality of reduced vowels is observed
    across languages
  • contextually variable vowel ( high in most
    contexts) in the absence of quality contrasts.
  • mid central vowel in contrast with higher vowels.

54
End
55
Schwa presence vs. absence
saw an(other)
saw n(othing)
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com