Examining the Association Between Language Delay and BehavioralEmotional Problems in Children Under - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 39
About This Presentation
Title:

Examining the Association Between Language Delay and BehavioralEmotional Problems in Children Under

Description:

For children age 5. Language problems are often 'comorbid' with other problems (Cohen, 2001) About 50% of children in mental ... Rescorla & Alley (2001) ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:130
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 40
Provided by: thomasma8
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Examining the Association Between Language Delay and BehavioralEmotional Problems in Children Under


1
Examining the Association Between Language Delay
and Behavioral/Emotional Problems in Children
Under Age 3
  • Leslie Rescorla
  • Professor of PsychologyBryn Mawr College
  • ASHA, 2007
  • rescorl_at_brynmawr.edu

2
Association Between Language Delay and Behavior
Problems in Children gt Age 5
  • For children gt age 5
  • Language problems are often comorbid with other
    problems (Cohen, 2001)
  • About 50 of children in mental health clinics
    have a language impairment
  • About 50 of children in speech/language clinics
    have a behavioral or emotional disorder

3
Association Between Language Delay and Behavior
Problems in Children Age 3
  • For children age 3
  • A link between language delay and behavior
    problems was found in the Dunedin (Silva, 1980)
    and London (Stevenson Richman, 1978) general
    population samples
  • However, these associations were reduced when
    children with low IQ and/or neurodevelopmental
    delays were excluded

4
Association Between Language Delay and Behavior
Problems in Children lt Age 3
  • Results with children lt 3 years are mixed
  • Seven studies have used the Language Development
    Survey (LDS) and the Child Behavior Checklist
    (CBCL/1.5-5)
  • 4 from general population samples
  • 3 from clinic samples
  • Will present these 7 studies in some depth, after
    summarizing findings from six other studies that
    used various other measures

5
Recruited Late Talker Sample (1)
  • Caulfield, Fischel, DeBaryshe, Whitehurst
    (1989) recruited 34 late talkers (LTs)
  • LTs were compared with typically developing (TD)
    peers matched on age, gender, SES, and receptive
    language
  • On Eyberg Behavior Inventory and an interview,
    mothers of LTs reported more shyness and more
    bedtime problems than mothers of peers
  • LTs were more resistant and difficult than peers
    in lab clean up situation

6
Recruited Late Talker Sample (2)
  • Paul, Spangle-Looney Dahm (1991) recruited 21
    LTs
  • 29 of the LTs had receptive language delay as
    well as expressive language delay
  • LTs were compared with peers matched on age,
    gender, and SES
  • 62 of LTs had socialization delays on the
    Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale
  • At follow-up 12 to 18 months later, about 50 of
    the LTs still had socialization delays on the
    Vineland

7
Urban General Population Sample (3)
  • Horwitz et al. (2003) identified LTs with the CDI
  • 15, 18, and 28 scored lt 10th percentile on
    the CDI in 24-29, 30-35, 36-39 month groups (N
    639)
  • LTs scored lower than TDs on Infant-Toddler
    Social-Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) Competence
    but not higher on Internalizing or Dysregulation
  • 30-39 group had higher Externalizing scores (OR
    4.2)
  • ITSEA problem scores did not predict language
    delay in logistic regression analysis
  • Low ITSEA Competence ( attention, compliance,
    imitation/play, mastery motivation, empathy,
    prosocial peer relations) significantly predicted
    language delay in logistic regression (OR 3.2)

8
Early Intervention Sample (4)
  • Irwin, Carter, Briggs-Gowan (2002) recruited a
    sample of 14 LTs from an Early Intervention
    program
  • LTs were matched with 14 TD children from a
    general population sample on age, sex, and
    ethnicity
  • LTs had higher scores than TDs on ITSEA
    Depression/Withdrawal and Social Relatedness
  • LTs also scored higher than TD children on CBCL
    Withdrawn
  • Fitzgerald (2003) suggests LTs included some
    children with PDD, although procedures to exclude
    children with PDD were described by Irwin et al.

9
General Population Twin Sample (5)
  • Plomin et al. (2002) study with 4,000 twin pairs
  • Correlations between CDI score and Rutter Total
    Problems at 2 -.05 for both genders, at 3
    -.13 (B) -.09 (G)
  • Correlations with nonverbal ability and Total
    Problems at 2 -15 (B) -.10 (G), at 3 -25
    (B) -.22 (G)
  • rs also low between CDI and TP for those scoring
    lt10th percentile on CDI (.10 at 2, .20 at 3)
  • At 2, those scoring lt10th percentile on CDI
    scored lt .20 SDs above the mean on Total Problems
  • At 3, those scoring lt10th percentile on CDI
    scored about .30 above the mean on Total Problems

10
Early Intervention Clinic Sample (6)
  • Ross Weinberg (2006) studied 109 children age
    18 to 36 months evaluated in an Early
    Intervention clinic
  • Children with both receptive and expressive
    language delays on PLS-3 had lower scores on
    Bayley Rating Scale Orientation/Engagement and
    Emotional/Regulation factors than children with
    expressive delay only or with normal language
  • Children with mixed language delays also had
    significantly more neurodevelopmental delays
  • Both mixed and expressive language delayed groups
    scored lower than TD children on the Vineland
    Socialization scale

11
Summary of the Six Studies
  • Associations between language delay and
    behavioral/emotional problems in children lt 3 are
    generally modest, if significant at all
  • Both internalizing and externalizing problems
    have shown some association with language delay
  • Associations between language delay and behavior
    problems increase as children approach age 3
  • Associations may be stronger between language
    delay and measures of socialization and adaptive
    behavior than measures of behavior problems
  • Selection factors may increase association
    between language delay and behavior problems in
    recruited LT samples, due to Berksons bias.
  • Next, review seven LDS/CBCL studies

12
Language Development Survey
  • Language Development Survey (LDS)
  • checklist completed by parents of children 18-35
    months
  • 310 vocabulary words arranged by semantic
    category
  • vocabulary score sum of words reported to be
    used by child spontaneously (not imitated or
    comprehended only).
  • Does child combine words into phrases?
  • If yes, parent writes in childs five longest
    best phrases
  • mean number of words calculated for the five
    phrases
  • LDS norms
  • Vocabulary score norms - separately by gender for
    18-23, 24-29, and 30-35 months age groups
  • Mean length of phrases norms - for 24-29 30-35
    months age groups with genders combined

13
(No Transcript)
14
(No Transcript)
15
Psychometric Properties of LDS
  • Reliability
  • Test-retest reliability .97-.99
  • Cronbachs alpha .99
  • Validity
  • Correlations with expressive language tests
    .66-.87
  • Sensitivity generally gt80, specificity gt 85
  • Predictive validity from age 2 (Rescorla, 2002)
    (N59)
  • age 7 vocabulary .63
  • age 8 grammar .41
  • age 8 age 9 reading .39 .34

16
Assessment of Behavioral and Emotional Problems
  • CBCL/1.5 - 5 and C-TRF
  • 99 problem items (0, 1, 2 scale) (not true,
    somewhat or sometimes true, very true or often
    true)
  • write in concerns and best things about the child
  • normed in general population sample
  • forms yield scores on empirically based
    syndromes, DSM-oriented scales, and
    Internalizing, Externalizing, and Total Problems

17
(No Transcript)
18
Syndromes for Preschool Forms
  • Emotionally Reactive
  • Anxious/Depressed
  • Somatic Complaints
  • Withdrawn
  • Attention Problems
  • Aggressive Behavior
  • Sleep Problems (CBCL only)

19
DSM-Oriented Scales for Preschool Forms
  • Affective Problems
  • Anxiety Problems
  • Pervasive Developmental Problems
  • Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Problems
  • Oppositional Defiant Problems

20
(No Transcript)
21
(No Transcript)
22
Psychometric Properties of the CBCL/1.5-5
  • Reliability
  • test-retest mean r .85 (.68 to .90 across
    scales)
  • internal consistency alphas .89 to .95 for INT,
    EXT, TP range from .63 to .92 for syndromes and
    DSM-oriented scales
  • Validity
  • Demographically-matched referred and nonreferred
    samples (N 563, per group) differed
    significantly on all CBCL scales and items
  • ES 22 for Total Problems, 20 for INT, 8 for
    EXT

23
(No Transcript)
24
(No Transcript)
25
LDS/CBCL Studies in General Population Samples (1)
  • Carson et al. (1998)
  • LTs (N 17) and TD peers (N 47) who were
    identified using the LDS in a mail-based
    screening in Wyoming were seen for assessment
  • LTs had higher CBCL Total Problems, Sleep
    Problems, and Other Problems scores than TD peers
  • But only 38 of possible LTs invited to
    participate came in for the evaluation which
    suggests a selection bias favoring children with
    more problems

26
LDS/CBCL Studies in General Population Samples (2)
  • Rescorla Alley (2001)
  • Assessed 33 LTs identified with the LDS in a
    household screening (80 yield rate), plus 33
    matched TD toddlers with normal language
  • LT and TD groups differed greatly on LDS
    Vocabulary (29 vs. 196 words) at screening and at
    assessment about 1 month later (43 vs. 230 words)
  • Large group differences on Bayley, Reynell, and
    Vineland Adaptive Behavior score (93 vs. 110)
  • No group differences on CBCL Total Problems

27
LDS/CBCL Studies in General Population Samples (3)
  • Rescorla Achenbach (2002)
  • Used LDS and CBCL data for 278 children in
    national survey (18-23, 24-29, 30-35 months)
  • Vocabulary and Mean Phrase Length scores not
    correlated with any CBCL scores (22 tests, p lt
    .001)
  • For ages 24 to 35 months, 15 had fewer than 50
    words or no combinations and 19 had CBCL TP
    scores gt 60 (borderline clinical range)
  • No significant association between LDS delay and
    CBCL deviance (only 6/172 deviant on both - 3)
  • None of 9 referred kids were deviant on both

28
LDS/CBCL Studies in General Population Samples (4)
  • Zubrick Western Australia household survey
  • 900 children 24 to 25 months screened with LDS
    and CBCL/1-5-5
  • 11 had fewer than 50 words VOC Delay
  • 10 had no combinations COMB Delay
  • 20 had fewer than 50 words OR no combinations
    LDS delay (lack of overlap between vocabulary and
    combinations unusual)
  • deviant on TP, INT, and Ext gt 84th ile
  • deviant on Withdrawn gt 93rd ile

29
Zubrick Results
  • LDS Vocabulary score not significantly correlated
    with any CBCL/1/5-5 scores
  • Children with VOC Delay did not have higher
    scores on any CBCL problem scale
  • Children with COMB Delay and LDS Delay had higher
    scores on Withdrawn (only CBCL scale with a
    significant difference)
  • LDS Delay not significantly associated with
    deviance on TP, INT, or EXT
  • Association with deviance on Withdrawn was modest
    (r .09) but significant (p lt .01)

30
LDS/CBCL Studies in Clinics New Jersey Sample
(5)
  • Sample of 83 children age 18 to 35 months
  • Study 1 in Rescorla, Ross, McClure (2007)
  • Children being assessed for developmental
    problems or as part of NICU follow-up
  • 64 of the sample were delayed on the LDS (lt15th
    percentile on Vocabulary or lt 20th percentile on
    Mean Phrase Length)
  • Mean Total Problems score 54.44 17 scored
    gt 84th percentile on TP ( deviant)
  • 31 scored gt 93rd percentile on DSM-Pervasive
    Developmental Problems (PDP) scale

31
New Jersey Sample Results
  • Correlational results with Vocabulary SS
  • Significant rs with CBCL Total Problems (-.38),
    Internalizing (-.44), Emotionally Reactive
    (-.24), and Withdrawn (-.56)
  • BUT with children deviant on PDP excluded, only
    Withdrawn still significant (r - .42)
  • Cross-tabulation of deviance on LDS/CBCL
  • Only 21 of children with language delay on LDS
    were deviant on TP not significant
  • But 79 of those deviant on TP had delayed
    language on LDS

32
LDS/CBCL Studies in Clinics New York Sample (6)
  • Sample of 103 children age 18 to 35 months
  • Study 2 in Rescorla, Ross, McClure (2007)
  • Essentially the same sample as Ross Weinberg
    (2006) Early Intervention clinic study
  • 7 were diagnosed with PDD, 47 had Bayley MDI
    scores lt75, and 30 were born premature
  • 56 were delayed in vocabulary on the LDS
  • 66 were language-delayed based on delay in
    either vocabulary or mean phrase length
  • Mean CBCL Total problems scores 48.53, 16 to
    17 scored in deviant range (gt 84th percentile)

33
New York Sample Results
  • Correlational results with Vocabulary SS
  • The only significant LDS/CBCL association was
    with Withdrawn (r - .20, p lt .05)
  • The other 9 correlations ranged from -.09 to .08
  • Cross-tabulation of deviance on LDS/CBCL
  • No significant associations between delay on LDS
    and deviance on Total Problems, INT, or EXT (phi
    coefficients .07 to .11)
  • Only 17 of children with language delay on LDS
    were deviant on INT

34
LDS/CBCL Studies in Clinics Referred Manual
Sample (7)
  • Sample of 102 children age 18 to 35 months
  • Children assessed in various developmental and
    mental health clinics
  • Referred children lt 3 years of age from referred
    sample in CBCL/1.5-5 manual
  • Significant rs between LDS Vocabulary and
    Withdrawn, Attention Problems, Other,
    Internalizing, Total Problems, DSM-Affective, and
    DSM-PDP scales (p lt .01, rs .25 to .35)

35
Referred Manual Sample (II)
  • Cross-tabulation of delay on LDS with deviance on
    CBCL problem scores
  • Significant ?2 associations (p lt .01) for
    Withdrawn (OR 4.8), Internalizing (OR 3.9),
    and Total Problems (OR 3.7)
  • Comorbid percentages
  • delayed on LDS also deviant on CBCL
  • TP 52 and INT 58 (T 60)
  • WITH 27 (T 65)
  • deviant on CBCL also delayed on LDS
  • TP 47 and INT 44 (T 60)
  • WITH 36 (T 65)

36
Conclusions Association Between Language Delay
and BehaviorProblems in Children lt Age 3
  • General population samples
  • Language delay on the LDS and deviance on the
    CBCL are not generally associated
  • Clinical samples
  • More association between language delay on the
    LDS and deviance on the CBCL
  • Most consistent association is for Withdrawn
  • Degree of association varies with type of clinic
    and composition of sample

37
Conclusions and Implications
  • Main conclusions
  • Many children lt3 with language delays have normal
    behavioral/emotional adjustment
  • Many children lt3 with behavioral/emotional
    maladjustment have normal language development
  • Children with both language delay and
    maladjustment are likely to have low IQs, high
    family risk, low SES, or PDD spectrum problems
  • Most common problem associated with language
    delay is social withdrawal
  • Children under age 3 with language delay tend to
    have delays in adaptive behavior/socialization

38
Explanatory Speculations
  • Insofar as behavioral/emotional problems are a
    consequence of language delay
  • it may take time for the negative effects of
    language delay to produce behavior problems
  • the negative effects of language delay may
    increase when children move into group settings
    where they have to interact more with peers
  • Insofar as language delay and behavior problems
    arise from shared genetic or environmental
    factors
  • the emergence of language delay may precede the
    emergence of behavioral/emotional problems

39
Questions for Further Research
  • Longitudinal studies are needed to examine the
    association between language delay and
    behavioral/emotional problems
  • Do LT toddlers develop more behavioral or
    emotional maladjustment as they get older?
  • Do LTs in day care settings develop behavioral
    problems earlier than LTs cared for at home?
  • Do LTs with normal receptive language have less
    maladjustment than LTs with mixed delays?
  • Do IQ and/or SES moderate the link between
    language delay and behavior problems?
  • What interventions can forestall development of
    behavior problems in children with language
    delay?
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com