Maternal Socialization of Emotion Through Narrative Storytelling With Preschoolers S' Kibblewhite, T - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

Maternal Socialization of Emotion Through Narrative Storytelling With Preschoolers S' Kibblewhite, T

Description:

Maternal Socialization of Emotion Through Narrative Storytelling With Preschoolers ... considering that, (1) participants were primed to the purpose of the task ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:89
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: web4Uw
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Maternal Socialization of Emotion Through Narrative Storytelling With Preschoolers S' Kibblewhite, T


1
Maternal Socialization of Emotion Through
Narrative Storytelling With PreschoolersS.
Kibblewhite, T. Lopetrone, S. Voelker, J.
Hakim-LarsonUniversity of Windsor
  • Abstract
  • The primary purpose of the present study was to
    examine normative emotion decoding data for a
    sample of self-reported emotion coaching mothers
    engaging in a didactic storytelling task. A
    second purpose of the study was to examine
    parenting style as a multidimensional construct
    in relation to emotion decoding. A total of 18
    mothers of preschool children completed the
    Parenting Styles Self-Test (PSST Gottman, 1997
    modified by Hakim-Larson Lee, 1999), the
    Reasons for Reading Questionnaire (adapted from
    Harkins, 1993), and a storytelling task using the
    Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969) picture book.
    Mothers spontaneously provided few emotion labels
    for the 16 emotion stimuli in the storytelling
    task. They were more accurate when decoding
    obvious versus subtle stimuli. Mothers overall
    decoding accuracy correlated positively with the
    importance they place in reading to their child
    to introduce emotionally difficult topics.
    Mothers disapproving scores correlated
    positively with negative emotion decoding
    accuracy. Mothers dismissing scores correlated
    positively with negative emotion decoding
    accuracy and negatively with positive emotion
    decoding accuracy. No significant differences in
    number of emotions labeled or decoding accuracy
    were found as a function of child gender. Study
    findings are discussed briefly.
  • Method
  • This study is part of a larger project examining
    the development of emotional competence within
    the context of parent-child interactions
  • Participants
  • 18 mothers of preschoolers recruited through an
    undergraduate Psychology participant pool
  • Mothers ranged in age from 24 to 38 (M 29.2, SD
    4.45)
  • Preschoolers (9 males, 9 females) ranged in age
    from 3 to 5 (M 4.0, SD .91) Childrens ages
    did not differ significantly by gender
  • All of the mothers identified primarily with an
    emotion coaching style
  • Results (continued)
  • Emotion Decoding Accuracy
  • Mothers were generally accurate in the emotion
    labels that they provided, M 87.65, SD 19.99
    (see Table 2)
  • Mothers were more accurate when decoding obvious
    versus subtle stimuli, t(11) 2.272, p lt .05
  • No significant differences in decoding accuracy
    were found as a function of child gender
  • Reasons for Reading Mothers overall decoding
    accuracy correlated positively with the
    importance they place in reading to their child
    to introduce emotionally difficult topics, r
    .557, p lt .05
  • Parenting Style Mothers disapproving scores
    correlated positively with negative emotion
    decoding accuracy, r .471, p lt .10. Mothers
    dismissing scores correlated positively with
    negative emotion decoding accuracy, r .485, p lt
    .10, and negatively with positive emotion
    decoding accuracy, r -.583, p lt .10
  • Discussion
  • Number Of Emotion Labels Provided
  • Mothers in general, provided few emotion labels
    for the emotion stimuli. This was somewhat
    surprising considering that, (1) participants
    were primed to the purpose of the task and its
    focus on emotion socialization, and (2) the
    sample primarily identified with an emotion
    coaching parenting style that would be
    hypothesized to value such an opportunity to
    teaching emotional understanding (Gottman, 1997).
    This study was limited by such a homogeneous
    sample. Future research may examine the
    prevalence of emotion labels provided across
    parenting styles. Furthermore, the absence of
    emotion labels does not necessarily imply the
    absence of emotional content. Examination of
    behavioural expressions of affect (e.g., smiling,
    crying) and emotional tone may also provide
    valuable information regarding socialization of
    emotion during a story-telling task.
  • Mothers laissez-faire scores correlated
    negatively with the overall number of emotion
    responses provided. These findings fit with the
    conceptualization of laissez-faire parents as
    those who take little opportunity to teach their
    children about emotions (Gottman, 1997). As
    such, they may be less likely to include emotion
    language into their storytelling.
  • Emotion Decoding Accuracy
  • The findings observed in which mothers were more
    accurate when decoding obvious versus subtle
    emotion stimuli parallels findings reported in
    the literature (e.g., Hess, Blairy, Kleck,
    1997). Emotion stimuli that more closely
    approximate prototypical expressions of emotion
    are generally easier to decode.
  • Mothers overall decoding accuracy correlated
    positively with the importance they place in
    reading to their child to introduce emotionally
    difficult topics, r .557, p lt .05. Perhaps
    those parents who value reading as an emotion
    socialization activity are more invested and
    deliberate when describing the emotion-related
    characteristics of story-book characters.
  • Mothers disapproving and dismissing scores
    correlated positively with negative emotion
    decoding accuracy. This has interesting
    implications for these parents who perceive
    themselves as primarily emotion coaching, but may
    be biased toward accuracy in teaching about
    negative emotions.
  • Child Gender
  • No significant differences in decoding accuracy
    or number of emotions labelled were found as a
    function of child gender. These findings do not
    correspond with studies of child gender and
    maternal discussion of emotion mentioned earlier.
    However, differences based on child gender may
    not be as salient in a sample of emotion coaching
    parents who value the opportunity to talk with
    and educate their children about emotions
    (Gottman, 1997).
  • Measures/Procedure
  • Background Information Form
  • Parenting Styles Self-Test (Gottman, 1997
    modified by Lee, Hakim-Larson, Voelker, 2000)
  • Reasons for Reading Questionnaire (adapted from
    Harkins, 1993)
  • Participants completed a storytelling task in
    their homes in which they tape-recorded
    themselves telling a text-free story to their
    child (Frog, Where Are You? Mayer, 1969)
  • Introduction
  • Research examining emotion socialization in
    families suggests that parents influence their
    childrens social and emotional development by
    how they approach emotionally laden information
    themselves and how they convey this information
    or knowledge to their children (Eisenberg,
    Cumberland, Spinrad, 1998)
  • Parents may use stories or narratives to convey
    the meaning of events to their children (Saarni,
    1999)
  • The act of storytelling provides an opportunity,
    in an everyday setting, for parents to facilitate
    the development of a childs emotional competence
    through the discussion of emotional content as it
    is presented in children's stories
  • One aspect of storytelling involves the labelling
    or decoding of emotions (e.g., Berman Slobin,
    1994 Harkins, 1993 Stavans, 1996) .
  • The extent to which parents label characters
    emotions and label them accurately may vary as a
    function of
  • Intensity of the emotion stimulus (Hess, Blairy,
    Kleck, 1997)
  • Valence of the emotion stimulus (i.e., positive
    vs. negative Fox et al., 2000 Biehl, Matsumoto,
    Ekman, Hearn, 1997)
  • Child gender (Dunn, Bretherton, Munn, 1987
    Fivush, 1989 Kuebli, Butler, Fivush, 1995
    Kuebli Fivush, 1992)
  • Emotion parenting style
  • Emotion Parenting Style predominant method or
    style parents use to teach their children to
    recognize, express, and control emotion (Gottman,
    1997)
  • Four parenting styles have been identified
    (Gottman, 1997)
  • Emotion-coaching (i.e., value emotions as
    opportunities for teaching emotional
    understanding)
  • Laissez-faire (i.e., permissive acceptance of
    childrens emotions with little teaching of
    emotional regulation and management)
  • Dismissing (i.e., minimization of emotions with
    little or no effort to teach the child emotional
    problem-solving)
  • Disapproving (i.e., criticism and punishment of
    childrens emotions with an overemphasis on
    conformity and limits)
  • Table 1
  • Mean Number of Emotion Labels (N 18)
  • Results
  • Preliminary Analyses
  • Tapes were coded for the emotion labels mothers
    assigned to 16 of the picture book characters
    (i.e., the emotion stimuli)
  • This study utilized normative data on the decoded
    emotions collected by the primary author for her
    Masters Thesis (Kibblewhite Hakim-Larson,
    2002)
  • The 16 emotion stimuli examined in the current
    study were selected on the basis of the levels of
    decoded emotion agreement found in the original
    normative sample
  • The normative data dictated the categorization of
    the emotion stimuli into four different groups
    obvious (8), subtle (8), positive (8), and
    negative (8)
  • Accuracy of the mothers emotion labels was
    assessed through comparison with the normative
    data (i.e., decoding accuracy was based on per
    cent of emotion labels provided by the mothers
    that matched those obtained in the normative
    sample)
  • Number of Emotion Labels
  • Mothers spontaneously provided few emotion labels
    for the 16 emotion stimuli in the story telling
    task, M 4.22, SD 2.98 (see Table 1)
  • Mothers provided the same number of emotion
    labels for both obvious and subtle emotion
    stimuli, t(17) .860, p .40
  • When controlled for the duration of the
    storytelling task, mothers laissez-faire scores
    correlated negatively with the overall number of
    emotion responses provided, pr -.72, p lt .05
  • No significant differences in number of emotions
    labeled were found as a function of child gender
  • Table 2
  • Mean Percentage of Decoding Accuracy (N 18)
  • References
  • Berman, R. A., Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating
    events in narrative A crosslinguistic
    developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence
    Erlbaum.
  • Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., Hearn, V.
    (1997). Matsumoto and Ekmans Japanese and
    Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion
    (JACFEE) Reliability data and cross-national
    differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21,
    3-21.
  • Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., Munn, P. (1987).
    Conversations about feeling states between
    mothers and their young children. Developmental
    Psychology, 23, 132-139.
  • Fivush, R. (1989) Exploring sex differences in
    the emotional content of mother-child
    conversations about the past. Sex Roles, 20,
    675-691.
  • Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R.,
    Pichler, A., Dutton, K. (2000) Facial
    expressions of emotion Are angry faces detected
    more efficiently? Cognition Emotion,14, 61-92.
  • Harkins, D. A. (1993). Parental goals and styles
    of storytelling. In J. Demick, K. Bursik
    (Eds.), Parental Development. Hillsdale, NJ
    Lawrence Erlbaum
  • Hess, U., Blairy, S., Kleck, R. E. (1997). The
    intensity of emotional facial expressions and
    decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal
    Behavior, 21, 241-257.
  • Kibblewhite, S., Hakim-Larson, J. (2003,
    April). Family expressiveness and emotion
    decoding in university students. Poster
    presented at the meeting of the Society for
    Research in Child Development, Tampa, USA.
  • Kuebli, J., Butler, S., Fivush, R. (1995).
    Mother-child talk about past emotions Relations
    of maternal language and child gender over time.
    Cognition Emotion, 9, 265-283.
  • Kuebli, J. Fivush, R. (1992). Gender
    differences in parent-child conversations about
    past emotions. Sex Roles, 27, 683-698.
  • Lee, C., Hakim-Larson, J., Voelker, S. (2000,
    June). The Parenting Styles Self-test
    Psychometric Properties. Poster presented at the
    annual meeting of the Canadian Psychological
    Association, Ottawa, Ontario.
  • Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? NY Dial.
  • Stavans, A. (1996). Development of parental
    narrative input. Journal of Narrative and Life
    History, 6, 253-280.
  • Purpose of Present Study
  • Examine normative emotion decoding data for a
    sample of self-reported emotion coaching mothers
    engaging in a didactic storytelling task
  • Examine parenting style as a multidimensional
    construct in relation to emotion decoding
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com