Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to Cognitive Ability Thomas Dohmen, Armin Falk, David Huffm

1 / 39
About This Presentation
Title:

Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to Cognitive Ability Thomas Dohmen, Armin Falk, David Huffm

Description:

Are Risk Aversion and Impatience. Related to Cognitive Ability? ... Risk aversion and impatience are crucial determinants of behavior in economic ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:110
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 40
Provided by: Iza51

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to Cognitive Ability Thomas Dohmen, Armin Falk, David Huffm


1
Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to
Cognitive Ability? Thomas Dohmen, Armin Falk,
David Huffman, Uwe SundeIZA Bonn and University
of Bonn01-07-07ESA World Meeting 2007LUISS
Rome
2
Motivation
  • Risk aversion and impatience are crucial
    determinants of behavior in economic models, as
    is cognitive ability.
  • Their importance is confirmed empirically
  • Measures of risk aversion/ impatience predict a
    wide range of important economic behaviors.
  • Cognitive ability matters for wages and other
    labor market outcomes.
  • Typically, risk aversion and impatience are also
    assumed to be independent of cognitive ability.
  • This assumption, however, has received relatively
    little attention in the empirical literature.

3
Psychology Literature
  • Several psychology studies find that higher
    cognitive ability is associated with greater
    patience, although some others find no
    relationship.
  • There has been little work on cognitive ability
    and risk-taking.
  • Limitations of psychological evidence
  • Typically studies have reported raw correlations,
    without controls for, e.g., education, income.
  • Studies have been restricted to small samples of
    young children, or students, as subjects.
  • Measures of risk aversion or impatience are often
    (but not always) hypothetical.

4
Behavioral Economics Literature
  • Two recent studies provide evidence on a link
    between impatience or willingness to take risk
    and achievement test scores, or other more direct
    measures of cognitive ability
  • Benjamin et al. (2005) conduct paid choice
    experiments with Chilean high school students,
    and relate choices to SAT scores.
  • High school students with higher math SAT scores
    are more likely to be risk neutral, and more
    likely to choose patiently.
  • This is true controlling for parental
    income/education.
  • Frederick (2006) relates various measures of
    cognitive ability to patience and risk taking
  • Higher cognitive ability associated with greater
    patience, and willingness to take risks in the
    gain domain (less willing in lotteries with
    losses).
  • Subjects are college students and adults.
  • Results are correlations without controls.

5
A Representative Field Experiment
  • This paper contributes to this literature, in a
    way that complements and extends previous
    research.
  • We provide the first evidence based on a
    representative sample
  • 1,012 adults living in Germany.
  • Roughly half participate in paid choice
    experiments measuring risk aversion.
  • Roughly half participate in paid experiments
    measuring impatience over an annual time horizon.
  • Experiments involve relatively high stakes.
  • All participants take two tests of cognitive
    ability, corresponding to sub-models of
    widely-used IQ test.
  • We address confounds that could affect our study,
    as well as previous studies, in a series of
    robustness checks.

6
Why is it important to answer this question?
  • Relevant for the specification of structural
    models that include both cognitive ability and
    preferences.
  • E.g., Heckman et al. (2006) are unusual in
    allowing the discount rate to be related to
    cognitive ability.
  • Our study can shed light on the value of this
    flexible approach, and suggest a direction of
    correlation.
  • Interpretation of reduced form models with
    cognitive ability on right-hand side but not risk
    aversion or impatience.
  • E.g., regressing schooling on cognitive ability,
    without patience, leads to omitted variable bias.

7
Why is it important to answer this question?
  • Important for understanding intergenerational
    transmission of preferences and socio-economic
    status.
  • IQ known to be transmitted from parents to
    children.
  • Risk aversion and patience are strongly
    correlated between parents and children.
  • Dohmen et al. (2006), Knowles and Postlewaite
    (2005).
  • Could transmission of IQ be a mechanism behind
    similar choices over time or under uncertainty?
  • Ramifications for policy interventions focused on
    improving child IQ.
  • Potential for important feedback effects between
    child IQ and preferences.

8
Data
9
Design
  • Data were collected between June 9th to July 4th,
    2005.
  • Our sample was drawn so as to be representative
    of the adult population living in Germany.
  • We used the same company that conducts the
    surveys for the German Socio-Economic Panel
    (SOEP).
  • As for the SOEP, interviewers start at a randomly
    chosen address, and then knock on doors.
  • Interviews conducted in subjects homes.
  • In total, our data include 1,012 participants.
  • The interview had two parts
  • First, subjects answered a questionnaire.
  • Second, subjects were asked to participate in
    either a lottery experiment or inter-temporal
    choice experiment.
  • Answers were typed into a laptop computer.

10
Design (continued)
  • A random device in CAPI software determined
    whether the subject was asked to participate in a
    lottery experiment or an inter-temporal choice
    experiment.
  • We deliberately had subjects participate in only
    one experiment to avoid order effects.

11
Cognitive Ability Measures
  • Each of our two measures is designed to
    correspond to a different sub-module of the
    Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (German
    version).
  • The WAIS is one of the most widely used IQ tests,
    with 6 verbal and 5 non-verbal sub-modules.
  • One sub-module involves matching numbers and
    symbols with a 90 second time limit.
  • Another sub-module involves a timed vocabulary
    test.
  • Previous research shows that our tests correlate
    well with these sub-modules, other WAIS modules,
    and other prominent IQ measures
  • (Lang et al., 2003 and 2007)

12
Symbol Correspondence Test
  • Subjects were posed with 9 unfamiliar symbols,
    each paired with one of the digits 1 to 9.
  • After brief instructions, subjects saw a new
    screen
  • Mapping from numbers to symbols at the top.
  • In the center of the screen, one of the symbols,
    and a box underneath in which the subject could
    type a number.
  • After a subject entered a number, a new screen
    would appear, with another symbol.
  • Subjects had 90 seconds to match as many as
    possible.
  • 105 declined to participate, and procedural
    problems arose in a couple of cases, leaving 905
    subjects with non-missing scores

13
Word Fluency Test
  • Subjects named as many animal names as they could
    in 90 seconds.
  • After each naming, the experimenter pressed a key
    indicating
  • Correct name
  • Repeated name
  • Incorrect name.
  • Pilots with taped interviews indicate that
    experimenter error rates are relatively low.
  • 87 declined to participate, a few more started
    and then decided to stop. Procedural problems
    arose in some cases, leaving 848 subjects with
    non-missing scores.

14
Distribution of Symbol Correspondence Scores
15
Distribution of Word Fluency Scores
16
Lottery Experiment
  • Subjects made 20 choices in a choice table.
  • The choice in each row was between a lottery and
    a safe option.
  • Lottery 300 or 0 Euros with equal probability.
  • Safe option X Euros, where X varies across 20
    choices.
  • Subjects knew that at the end, one row would be
    randomly selected, choice in that row
    implemented.
  • Switching row in the choice table is an incentive
    compatible measure of certainty equivalent.
  • Switching later in the table indicates a greater
    willingness to take risks.

17
Choices in the Lottery Experiment
18
Inter-Temporal Choice Experiment
  • Subjects made 20 choices in a choice table.
  • The choice was always between a payment available
    Today and a larger amount available in one
    year.
  • Early payment 100 Euros.
  • Delayed payment 100X Euros, where X varies
    across 20 choices.
  • Subjects knew that at the end one row would be
    randomly selected, choice in that row
    implemented.
  • The payment would arrive in the mail as a check,
    cashable either immediately or in one year.
  • Switching row in the table measures impatience
    the rate of return needed to induce waiting one
    year.
  • Switching later indicates greater impatience.

19
Choices in the Inter-Temporal Choice Experiment
20
Results
21
Behavior and Symbol Correspondence Scores
22
Behavior and Word Fluency Scores
23
Baseline Regressions Willingness to Take Risks
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
24
Baseline Regressions Willingness to Take Risks
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
25
Baseline Regressions Impatience
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
26
Baseline Regressions Impatience
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
27
Robustness
28
Education
  • Cognitive ability is associated with achievement
    of more advanced schooling degrees.
  • This is true in our data, and in the literature.
  • Education, in turn, might affect risk aversion or
    impatience.
  • Thus, it is interesting to see whether cognitive
    ability has a direct effect, or works only
    indirectly through education.
  • Another issue arises because education could
    affect cognitive ability (e.g., Cascio and Lewis,
    2006).
  • Is cognitive ability simply a proxy for education
    in the baseline results?

29
Risk Results are Robust to Adding Education
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
30
Impatience Results Robust to Adding Education
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
31
Income and Credit Constraints
  • Cognitive ability is associated with higher
    income.
  • Income could have an impact on risk-taking
    behavior, or potentially patience.
  • In the lottery experiment, income cushions the
    impact of losing a lottery.
  • In the inter-temporal choice experiment, low
    income could imply credit constraints, making the
    choice100 Euros today a necessity.
  • It is interesting to see whether cognitive
    ability only works through the indirect channel
    of income.
  • Also, we use a survey measure of credit
    constraints
  • If you suddenly encountered an unforeseen
    situation, and had to pay an expense of 1,000
    Euros in the next two weeks, would you be able to
    borrow the money?

32
Risk Results are Robust to Adding Income
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
33
Impatience Results are Robust to Adding Income
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
34
Confusion about Incentives?
  • One potential confound could arise if low
    cognitive ability caused people to be confused
    about incentives.
  • This would be a problem if confusion lead to
    behavior that happened to look like, e.g.,
    greater risk aversion.
  • For risk attitudes we have a way to address this
    issue explicitly.
  • We use a very simple survey measure that asks
    about willingness to take risks
  • In general, are you a person who is fully
    prepared to take risks, or do you try to avoid
    taking risks?
  • Respondents answer on a scale for 0 to 10, where
    0 indicates completely unwilling and 10
    completely willing

35
Cognitive Ability also Related to Survey Measure
Notes Interval regression coefficients, marginal
effects. Robust s.e..
36
Other Robustness Checks
  • Could Test Scores Proxy for Personality?
  • Arbitrage between experiment and market rates of
    return.
  • Risk aversion, impatience, and test-taking
    strategy.
  • Time preference vs. concavity of utility (risk
    aversion) as determinants of impatient behavior.

37
Conclusions
  • We find a significant and robust relationship
    between cognitive ability, risk aversion, and
    impatience.
  • People with higher cognitive ability are more
    patient, and more willing to take risks.
  • Regardless of the precise mechanism, it is
    important to know that cognitive ability, risk
    aversion, and impatience are systematically
    related.
  • Implications for specifying econometric models.
  • Interpretation of reduced form models.
  • Intergenerational transmission.
  • Inequality and IQ.
  • Implications for policy interventions focused on
    improving child IQ.

38
Conclusions
  • For future research establishing underlying
    mechanisms.
  • Bounded rationality, in the sense of choice
    bracketing?
  • Cognitive ability could affect whether people
    bracket narrowly, making risky choices in
    isolation, or recognize that they form part of a
    larger portfolio.
  • Cognitive ability could also affect whether
    people integrate present and future
    considerations.
  • Two-system explanation?
  • Cognitive system in the brain can be overpowered
    by affective system affective system is the
    source of urges for immediate consumption, fear
    of losses.
  • Cognitive ability could proxy for the resources
    an individual has to suppress emotional urges.
  • Feedback from preferences to development of
    cognitive skills?
  • Although cognitive ability may affect risk
    aversion and impatience, there could also be
    important feedback effects.
  • E.g., patience conducive to developing cognitive
    skills.
  • Evolution
  • Adaptive for low-cognitive ability to go with
    conservative strategies of risk aversion and
    grabbing immediate rewards?

39
Thomas Dohmen IZA, P.O. Box 7240, 53072 Bonn,
Germany Phone 49 (0) 228 - 38 94 -204 Fax 49
(0) 228 - 38 94 180 E-mail dohmen_at_iza.org http/
/www.iza.org
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)