DISCOURSE TYPES, GENRE SCHEMATA, AND RHETORICAL RELATIONS - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

DISCOURSE TYPES, GENRE SCHEMATA, AND RHETORICAL RELATIONS

Description:

DISCOURSE TYPES, GENRE SCHEMATA, AND RHETORICAL RELATIONS Andrej A. Kibrik Institute of Linguistics, Russian Academy of Sciences kibrik_at_comtv.ru – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:422
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 39
Provided by: pyx142
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: DISCOURSE TYPES, GENRE SCHEMATA, AND RHETORICAL RELATIONS


1
DISCOURSE TYPES, GENRE SCHEMATA, AND
RHETORICAL RELATIONS
  • Andrej A. Kibrik
  • Institute of Linguistics,
  • Russian Academy of Sciences
  • kibrik_at_comtv.ru

2
Genres and discourse types
  • Discourse studies typology of discourse
    specimens is the least developed area (cf. e.g.
    van Dijk ed. 1997)
  • However, the issue is important in any empirical
    discipline a classification of specimens is among
    central tasks

3
General problem
  • In modern discourse studies, there is no
    satisfactory classification of genres or
    discourse types
  • All available classifications are purely
    enumerative
  • Enumerative inventories cannot be demonstrated to
    be exhaustive and internally coherent

4
A classification of animals in a Chinese
Encyclopediacited in Borges Celestial Emporium
of Benevolent Knowledge
  • those that belong to the Emperor,
  • embalmed ones,
  • suckling pigs,
  • those that are trained,
  • mermaids,
  • fabulous ones,
  • stray dogs,
  • those included in the present classification,
  • those that tremble as if they were mad,
  1. innumerable ones,
  2. those drawn with a very fine camelhair brush,
  3. others,
  4. those that have just broken a flower vase,
  5. those that from a long way off look like flies

5
General goal
  • Move towards a non-enumerative, but rather a
    calculus-type classification of discourse genres

6
Specific problem
  • Discourse genres are defined as non-linguistic
    phenomena
  • Swales (1990)
  • genres are attributes of discourse communities
  • genres serve typical communicative intentions
    salient in such communities
  • It is not clear whether genres can be attributed
    any linguistic properties

7
Specific goals
  • Address the question are discourse genres
    linguistically identifiable?
  • If yes, attempt to provide a linguistic
    definition of at least one genre

8
ATTEMPTS OF LINGUISTIC GENRE DEFINITION
  • Linguistic definition 1 Genre schemata
  • TOO GENERAL
  • Linguistic definition 2 Morphosyntactic and
    lexical features
  • TOO DETAILED
  • An alternative linguistic definition
    Configurations of rhetorical structures

9
Linguistic definition 1 Genre schemata
  • The story schema (Chafe 1994)
  • Orientation
  • Complication
  • Climax
  • Denouement
  • Coda
  • Additional elements in Labov 1972
  • Abstract
  • Evaluation

10
Definition 1 (another example)
  • The Native English business letter schema (Kong
    1998)
  • Source of reference
  • Making the request
  • Background of the company
  • Justification for the request
  • Stating the conditions
  • Other related requests
  • Cordial conclusion

11
Definition 1 is problematic
  • Too large-scale approach It is unclear how one
    can make any predictions of the linguistic form
    of a genre specimen

12
Linguistic definition 2 Morphosyntactic and
lexical features
  • Biber 1989
  • 481 texts in corpus
  • 67 morphosyntactic and lexical features
  • 5 dimensions groups of covarying features
  • 8 clusters of texts in the 5-dimensional space
  • 8 text types with tentative labels, such as
    intimate interpersonal interaction
  • limited correlation to established genres
  • Example 62 texts of the genre of personal
    telephone conversation belong to the text type
    intimate interpersonal interaction
  • The conclusion is that genres are linguistically
    irrelevant

13
Linguistic definition 2 fails, as demonstrated by
Biber
  • Too small-scale approach Individual
    morphosyntactic and lexical features are
    incommensurable with discourse genres as wholes
  • But why do discourses of the same type fail to
    have consistent characteristics?

14
A possible clue types of passages
  • Narrative
  • Descriptive
  • Expository (explanatory)
  • Instructive and hortatory
  • Persuasive (argumentative)
  • (see e.g. Longacre 1992)
  • this list is enumerative, too, but at least the
    number of categories is more embraceable

15
Selected features of discourse passages
16
Reasons for Bibers results
  • Morphosyntactically and lexically identifiable
    discourse units are passages rather than
    discourses as wholes
  • Genres are not internally homogeneous in terms of
    passage types they consist of more than one
    passage type
  • Therefore, discourses as wholes cannot be
    expected to be consistent in terms of
    morphosyntactic and lexical features

17
A set of working hypotheses
  • So, the question is what could be a viable
    linguistic definition of discourse genres? Or at
    least of passage types?
  • Prerequisite Genres can be defined in terms of
    genre schemata
  • Genres schemata can be defined as combinations of
    passage types
  • Passage types can be defined in terms of
    rhetorical relations
  • Therefore, genres can also be ultimately defined
    in rhetorical terms

18
A CASE STUDY
  • THE NIGHT DREAM STORIES PROJECT
  • Original goal search for differences in
    discourse structure in the night dream stories of
    children with and without neurotic disorders
  • 69 stories from neurotic children
  • 60 stories from neurologically intact children
  • About 3000 discourse units in corpus
  • The corpus has been
  • transcribed
  • RST-diagrammed

19
Rhetorical structure theory (RST)
  • Originally formulated by Mann and Thompson 1988
  • A unified view of discourse structure,
    irrespective of the size of discourse segments
  • A nomenclature of rhetorical relations between
    discourse segments
  • Each discourse segment serves the realization of
    the overall communicative intention of the
    speaker
  • We added a number of rhetorical relations to the
    canonical set in order to account for narrative
    discourse data (Kibrik, Podlesskaya, Kalkova,
    and Litvinenko 2002)

20
Generalized schema of a night dream story
  • Begin
  • (Headline)
  • Setting
  • NARRATIVE CHAIN
  • (Evaluation)
  • (Summary)
  • End

21
Two major types of passages in stories
  • The great majority of texts in corpus are
    predominantly narrative ( are stories)
  • 129 texts altogether
  • 6 non-narrative texts
  • Narrative chain Narrative type of passage
  • Setting Descriptive type of passage

22
Typical normal story Z11
  • 1. My s klassom ..(1.8) poshli ..(1.1) vot
    ..(0.5) kuda-to.
  • My classmates and I went somewhere.
  • 2. ..(0.3) Zashli v dom,
  • Entered a house,
  • 3. ..(1.2) i tam ..(0.2) byli stupen'ki ..(1.8)
    i voda.
  • and there were steps and water there.
  • 4. ..(1.0) My stali na bol'shoj plot,
  • We went onto a big raft
  • 5. ..(0.6) i pereexali na druguju storonu.
  • and crossed to the other side.
  • 6. ..(1.5) Potom ..(1.4) my vyshli iz dveri.
  • Then we exited the door.
  • 7. ..(0.8) Tam byla dver' ...(1.0) takaja
    zheltaja.
  • There was a door there, a yellow one.
  • 8. ..(0.5) My otkryli ee,
  • We opened it,
  • ...

23
RST-diagram of text Z11
24
RHETORICAL STRUCTURE OF NARRATIVE PASSAGES
  • Uppermost relations
  • Sequence
  • Consequence
  • (Emotional reaction)
  • (Discord)
  • All these relations are variants of the basic
    narrative relation
  • We can therefore define the narrative passage as
    a passage that has one of narrative relations in
    its uppermost node

25
Typical neurotic story N08
  • 1. Ja byla doma ..(0.3) s mamoj, ..(1.1) s
    bratom, I was at home with my mom, with my
    brother,
  • 2. ..(0.4) nu tam ..(0.3) kot mne eshche snilsja
    moj. well I dreamt about my cat too.
  • 3. ...(2.8 m) Dolgoe tam vremja snilos', For a
    long time I dreamt
  • 4. kak my prosto doma tam, how we were just at
    home
  • 5. delami zanimaemsja. doing various chores.
  • 6. ..(1.) Potom ..(0.2) chego-to ..(0.2) trevogu
    ja pochuvstvovala, Then for some reason I felt
    anxiety,
  • 7. vygljanula v okno, looked out of the window,
  • 8. u nashego pod'ezda pozharnaja mashina
    stoit. next to our entrance there was a fire
    engine.
  • ...

26
RST-diagram of text N08
27
Frequency of the uppermost narrative relations
Relations n
Sequence 91 71
Consequence 29 22
Emotional reaction 3 2
NONE (non-narrative text) 6 5
TOTAL 129 100
28
Relations appearing above narrative relations
  • Begin
  • Headline
  • Setting
  • (Evaluation)
  • (Summary)
  • End
  • Out-relations
  • All these relations are genre-organizational
    for the genry of story

29
Frequency of the highest level non-narrative
relations
Relations above narrative Number
NONE 20
Begin 9
End 69
Headline 10
Summary 1
Setting 55
Out-relations 13
30
Rhetorical relations-based definition of the
genre of story
  • Story is a discourse that has one of narrative
    relations in its highest node in the rhetorical
    graph, with the exclusion of genre-organizational
    relations Begin, End, Headline, Summary, Setting,
    and Evaluation

31
A generalized rhetorical diagram of a story

32
RHETORICAL STRUCTURE OF DESCRIPTIVE PASSAGES IN
STORIES (SETTINGS) Frequency of settings in
stories
Z N Total
Setting 25 38 63 49
No Setting 35 31 66 51
Total 60 69 129 100
33
Most typical relations appearing at the uppermost
node of descriptive passages
  • Joint
  • Elaboration
  • Background
  • At a certain degree of granularity, these three
    relations can be taken as varieties of one and
    the same

34
Relations that can potentially appear above the
typical descriptive relations
  • Source-out is the only relation that appears in
    this position in corpus more than once
  • Five other relations appear once each, most of
    them of organizational (e.g. Summary) or
    realizational (e.g. Split) kind

35
Frequency of settings by structural type
Uppermost relation n n, including similar
TRIVIAL CASE 14 14 22
Joint 13 16 25
Source-out gt Joint 3
Elaboration 15 22 35
Source-out gt Elaboration 4
Background 4 6 10
Source-out gt Background 2
Other 5 5 8

TOTAL 63 100
36
Generalizations on the rhetorical structure of
descriptive passages
  • There is a significant portion (22) of trivial
    descriptive passages that do not contain any
    rhetorical relation
  • Whenever descriptive passages are not trivial,
    they can be reliably defined as having one of the
    relations Joint, Elaboration, and Background in
    their uppermost node

37
Questions remaining for future research
  • Are descriptive passages as well rhetorically
    definable as narrative passages?
  • Are basic descriptive relations used exclusively
    in descriptive passages?
  • Can it be the case that types of passages are
    rhetorically definable only when they figure as
    major (Longacre) types of passages in
    particular discourse genres?
  • If that is the case, rhetorical definitions may
    be more suitable for genres than passage types.
  • How can one define trivial passages? Perhaps
    inherent aspect of predicates?
  • Can all discourse material be attributed to a
    certain type of passage?

38
CONCLUSIONS
  • At least some passage types can be identified in
    terms of rhetorical relations
  • Since genre schemata can be defined as
    configurations of passage types, genres can also
    be ultimately defined in terms of rhetorical
    relations
  • A rhetorical relations-based definition appears
    adequate for the narrative type of passage, and
    for the discourse genre of story
  • For descriptive passages in stories, a rhetorical
    definition is useful when such passages are not
    trivial
  • The question of whether a rhetorical definition
    can be universally used to define discourse
    passages and genres requires further study
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com