Differential Susceptibility to Family Conflict: The Interaction Effect of Temperament - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 54
About This Presentation
Title:

Differential Susceptibility to Family Conflict: The Interaction Effect of Temperament

Description:

Differential Susceptibility to Family Conflict: The Interaction Effect of Temperament Parenting on Early Childhood Delinquent Symptoms Xiao Zhang – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:257
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 55
Provided by: XiaoZ3
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Differential Susceptibility to Family Conflict: The Interaction Effect of Temperament


1
Differential Susceptibility to Family Conflict
The Interaction Effect of Temperament
Parenting on Early Childhood Delinquent Symptoms
  • Xiao Zhang
  • Department of Psychology
  • University of Jyväskylä

2
Background
  • Part of a longitudinal project of Chinese
    children during the transition to nursery care

3
Background
  • Part of a longitudinal project of Chinese
    children during the transition to nursery care
  • Transition to child care
  • ---inherently stressful for young children
    (unfamiliar adults, unknown peers, and new
    physical settings for extended periods of time in
    absence of their parents)

4
Background
  • Part of a longitudinal project of Chinese
    children during the transition to nursery care
  • Transition to child care
  • ---inherently stressful for young children
    (unfamiliar adults, unknown peers, and new
    physical settings for extended periods of time in
    absence of their parents)
  • ---increased risk of
    psychopathology symptoms across cultures (Ahnert
    et al., 2004 Klein et al., 2010 Zhang Sun,
    2010)

5
Background
  • Part of a longitudinal project of Chinese
    children during the transition to nursery care
  • Transition to child care
  • ---inherently stressful for young children
    (unfamiliar adults, unknown peers, and new
    physical settings for extended periods of time in
    absence of their parents)
  • ---increased risk of
    psychopathology symptoms across cultures (Ahnert
    et al., 2004 Klein et al., 2010 Zhang Sun,
    2010)
  • Initial intention Family School ?
    Psychopathology
  • (Temperament
    covariate)

6
Background
  • Part of a longitudinal project of Chinese
    children during the transition to nursery care
  • Transition to child care
  • ---inherently stressful for young children
    (unfamiliar adults, unknown peers, and new
    physical settings for extended periods of time in
    absence of their parents)
  • ---increased risk of
    psychopathology symptoms across cultures (Ahnert
    et al., 2004 Klein et al., 2010 Zhang Sun,
    2010)
  • Initial intention Family School ?
    Psychopathology
  • (Temperament
    covariate)
  • Aim of this paper Temperament Family ?
    Psychopathology

7
Literature Review
8
Family Conflict
  • Characterized by marital discord and more general
    anger, hostility, and criticism among family
    members.

9
Family Conflict
  • Characterized by marital discord and more general
    anger, hostility, and criticism among family
    members.
  • A marker of dysfunctional families (Cummings,
    1994).
  • ---associated with elevated risk for a wide
    range of psychopathology symptoms, including
    internalizing (e.g., withdrawal, depression,
    anxiety) and externalizing (e.g., aggression,
    delinquency) (see a review in Kelly, 2000)

10
Family Conflict
  • Not all children who experience conflictual and
    harsh home environments develop symptoms of
    psychopathology (Garmezy et al., 1984).

11
Family Conflict
  • Not all children who experience conflictual and
    harsh home environments develop symptoms of
    psychopathology (Garmezy et al., 1984).
  • A number of child characteristics have been
    suggested to moderate (either exacerbate or
    attenuate) the risk of stressful family
    environments

12
Family Conflict
  • Not all children who experience conflictual and
    harsh home environments develop symptoms of
    psychopathology (Garmezy et al., 1984).
  • A number of child characteristics have been
    suggested to moderate (either exacerbate or
    attenuate) the risk of stressful family
    environments
  • ---behavioral or temperamental in character
    (e.g., difficult/easy temperament), physiological
    or endophenotypic in nature (e.g., physiological
    reactivity), and genetic in origin (e.g.,
    5-HTTLPR alleles) (Belsky Pluess, 2009)

13
Temperament
  • Biologically based individual differences in
    reactivity and self-regulation, influenced over
    time by heredity, maturation, and experience
    (Rothbart Bates, 1998).

14
Temperament
  • Biologically based individual differences in
    reactivity and self-regulation, influenced over
    time by heredity, maturation, and experience
    (Rothbart Bates, 1998).
  • Difficult temperament
  • ---operationalized in many different ways
    (e.g., high activity, negative emotionality)
  • ---repeatedly linked to the development of
    internalizing and externalizing symptoms during
    the preschool period (Fagan, 1990)

15
Temperament
  • Biologically based individual differences in
    reactivity and self-regulation, influenced over
    time by heredity, maturation, and experience
    (Rothbart Bates, 1998).
  • Difficult temperament
  • ---operationalized in many different ways
    (e.g., high activity, negative emotionality)
  • ---repeatedly linked to the development of
    internalizing and externalizing symptoms during
    the preschool period (Fagan, 1990)
  • Yet recent research suggests that the combination
    of difficult temperament with stressful family
    environments (or the Temperament Environment
    interaction) often has the greatest impact on
    psychopathology symptoms (e.g., Ramos et al.,
    2005 Tchann et al., 1996 Whiteside-Mansell et
    al., 2009).

16
Temperament Family Conflict
17
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Theoretical models (Temperament Environment)
  • ---Diathesis-stress hypothesis (Monroe
    Simons, 1991)
  • ---Differential susceptibility hypothesis
    (Belsky Pluess, 2009)

18
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Theoretical models (Temperament Environment)
  • ---Diathesis-stress hypothesis (Monroe
    Simons, 1991) children with difficult
    temperament are more vulnerable to stressful
    environments, showing more symptoms of
    psychopathology, than those with easy temperament
  • ---Differential susceptibility hypothesis
    (Belsky Pluess, 2009)

19
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Theoretical models (Temperament Environment)
  • ---Diathesis-stress hypothesis (Monroe
    Simons, 1991) children with difficult
    temperament are more vulnerable to stressful
    environments, showing more symptoms of
    psychopathology, than those with easy temperament
  • ---Differential susceptibility hypothesis
    (Belsky Pluess, 2009) difficult children who
    are most adversely affected by an environmental
    stressor are the very same ones who reap the most
    benefit from environmental support and
    enrichment, including the absence of adversity

20
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Theoretical models (Temperament Environment)

21
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Both hypotheses have received empirical support.

22
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Both hypotheses have received empirical support.
  • ---Diathesis-stress hypothesis In a
    cross-sectional study of 145 preschool children,
    children with difficult temperament (e.g., high
    activity, negative mood) in high-conflict
    families had the most internalizing and
    externalizing symptoms, whereas children with
    easy temperament had the fewest symptoms
    regardless of levels of family conflict (Tschann
    et al., 1996)

23
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Both hypotheses have received empirical support.
  • ---Diathesis-stress hypothesis In a
    cross-sectional study of 145 preschool children,
    children with difficult temperament (e.g., high
    activity, negative mood) in high-conflict
    families had the most internalizing and
    externalizing symptoms, whereas children with
    easy temperament had the fewest symptoms
    regardless of levels of family conflict (Tschann
    et al., 1996)
  • ---Differential susceptibility hypothesis
    In a longitudinal study of 728 children from ages
    1 to 8, children with difficult temperament
    (e.g., inadaptability, unpredictability) were
    more likely to develop externalizing symptoms as
    a result of high conflict in the family but also
    tended to show fewer symptoms in low-conflict
    families than those with easy temperament
    (Whiteside-Mansell et al., 2009)

24
Temperament Family Conflict
  • No support for either hypothesis

25
Temperament Family Conflict
  • No support for either hypothesis In a
    longitudinal study of 337 children from ages 5 to
    17, no interaction of temperament and marital
    conflict was found in the prediction of
    internalizing or externalizing problems (Leve et
    al., 2005).

26
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Possible Sources of the mixed results

27
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Possible Sources of the mixed results
  • ---reporter bias and shared method
    invariance the same informant to measure both
    temperament and symptoms

28
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Possible Sources of the mixed results
  • ---reporter bias and shared method
    invariance the same informant to measure both
    temperament and symptoms
  • ---hard to compare the results across
    studies difficult temperament was almost always
    conceptualized in general senses (instead of
    using specific dimensions) but operationalized in
    different ways (i.e., using different measures)

29
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Possible Sources of the mixed results
  • ---reporter bias and shared method
    invariance the same informant to measure both
    temperament and symptoms
  • ---hard to compare the results across
    studies difficult temperament was almost always
    conceptualized in general senses (instead of
    using specific dimensions) but operationalized in
    different ways (i.e., using different measures)
  • ---direction of causality some studies
    assessed conflict, temperament, and symptoms
    concurrently

30
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Thus, more studies are needed with (a)
    measurement of conflict, temperament, and
    behavior using different informants, (b)
    assessment of specific temperamental dimensions,
    and (c) controls for prior symptoms when
    examining the Temperament Family Conflict
    interaction in the prediction of later symptoms.

31
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Moreover, the extent to which findings derived
    from studies of Western children apply to Chinese
    children during a critical transitional period is
    not clear.

32
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Moreover, the extent to which findings derived
    from studies of Western children apply to Chinese
    children during a critical transitional period is
    not clear.
  • ---Chinese childrens symptoms of
    psychopathology are associated with both
    temperament (e.g., shyness-sensitivity Chen et
    al., 1992, 2009a behavioral inhibition Chen et
    al., 2009b effortful control, positive
    emotionality, anger-irritability Zhou et al.,
    2009) and family environments (Phillips et al.,
    1991). Yet we do not know whether family
    environments interact with temperament to
    influence symptoms among Chinese children.

33
Temperament Family Conflict
  • Moreover, the extent to which findings derived
    from studies of Western children apply to Chinese
    children during a critical transitional period is
    not clear.
  • ---Chinese childrens symptoms of
    psychopathology are associated with both
    temperament (e.g., shyness-sensitivity Chen et
    al., 1992, 2009a behavioral inhibition Chen et
    al., 2009b effortful control, positive
    emotionality, anger-irritability Zhou et al.,
    2009) and family environments (Phillips et al.,
    1991). Yet we do not know whether family
    environments interact with temperament to
    influence symptoms among Chinese children.
  • ---Symptoms of psychopathology during the
    transition to child care are affected by both
    environmental factors such as parenting (Klein et
    al., 2010) and child temperament (e.g.,
    approach/withdrawal, adaptability Ahnert et al.,
    2004). However, we do not know whether symptoms
    during this transition can be predicted by the
    Temperament Family Conflict interactions.

34
The Present Study
  • The present study extends previous work by
    examining the Temperament Family Conflict
    interaction in the prediction of delinquent
    symptoms among Chinese children during a critical
    developmental periodthe transition from home to
    child care.

35
The Present Study
  • The present study extends previous work by
    examining the Temperament Family Conflict
    interaction in the prediction of delinquent
    symptoms among Chinese children during a critical
    developmental periodthe transition from home to
    child care.
  • To this end, we followed up a group of children
    in Beijing during their one-year transition to
    nursery care.

36
The Present Study
  • Hypothesis a significant Temperament Family
    Conflict interaction

37
The Present Study
  • Hypothesis a significant Temperament Family
    Conflict interaction
  • ---this interaction is expected to be
    accounted for by the differential susceptibility
    hypothesis. Specifically, children with difficult
    temperament were expected to be more susceptible
    to the negative consequences of high conflict in
    the family, showing more delinquent symptoms, and
    also more positively affected by low conflict,
    showing fewer symptoms, than children with easy
    temperament.

38
Method
39
Method
  • Participants
  • ---At 3 months after nursery entry (Time 1
    ?T1?), 118 children (56 boys, 62 girls M 33.3
    months old, SD 3.1 months) participated with
    mothers, fathers, and head caregivers.
  • ---At 9 months after T1 (Time 2 ?T2? i.e.,
    the end of nursery care), 11 children had
    transferred to other nurseries and did not
    participate. Attrition analyses did not reveal
    significant differences in the childs age, sex,
    delinquent symptoms, temperament, or family
    conflict at T1.

40
Method
  • Measures

41
Method
  • Measures
  • ---Delinquent Symptoms at T1 and T2 via
    caregiver report using the delinquency subscale
    in Achenbachs (1992) Child Behavior
    Checklist/23 (CBCL/23). Alpha coefficients were
    0.75 and 0.79 for delinquent problems at T1 and
    T2, respectively. Higher scores indicate more
    symptoms.

42
Method
  • Measures
  • ---Delinquent Symptoms at T1 and T2 via
    caregiver report using the delinquency subscale
    in Achenbachs (1992) Child Behavior
    Checklist/23 (CBCL/23). The scale measures the
    extent to which children do not do what social
    rules require. Alpha coefficients were 0.75 and
    0.79 for delinquent problems at T1 and T2,
    respectively. Higher scores indicate more
    symptoms.
  • ---Family Conflict At T1 via father report
    using the conflict subscale in Moos and Mooss
    (1986) Family Environment Scale (FES). The scale
    measures the amount of openly expressed anger and
    conflict among family members. The alpha
    coefficient was 0.67. Higher scores indicate a
    conflictual environment in the family.

43
Method
  • Measure
  • ---Temperament at T1 via mother reports
    using the activity level, rhythmicity, and
    approach/withdrawal subscales in Thomas and
    Chesss (1977) Child Temperament Questionnaire
    (CTQ). Approach/withdrawal reflects a childs
    tendency to approach new people and situations.
    Activity level measures a child's general level
    of energy and movement. Rhythmicity reflects the
    degree of predictability of the timing of the
    childs biological functions such as hunger,
    sleep-wake cycles, and elimination. Alpha
    coefficients were 0.69 for the rhythmicity scale,
    0.67 for the approach-withdrawal scale, and 0.63
    for the activity level scale. Lower scores in
    rhythmicity and approach/withdrawal and higher
    scores in activity level indicate difficult
    temperament.

44
Results
  • Hierarchical regression to predict T2 delinquent
    symptoms (covariates T1 delinquent symptoms,
    maternal education, childs age and sex)

Predictors B (S.E.) ß ?R2
Family conflict .38 (.20) .17 .02
Activity level .06 (.24) .02
Rhythmicity .37 (.22) .16 .05
Approach/withdrawal .60 (.23) .26
Activity level Conflict .46 (.22) .21
Rhythmicity Conflict .36 (.26) .14 .05
Approach/withdrawal Conflict .28 (.21) .13
45
Results
B.51, p lt .01b
B.40, p lt .05b
46
Discussion
  • Consistent with the diathesis-stress hypothesis,
    the results showed that children with difficult
    temperament (i.e., high activity level) were more
    vulnerable to family conflict and showed more
    delinquent symptoms as compared to children with
    relatively easy temperament (i.e., low activity
    level).

47
Discussion
  • Consistent with the diathesis-stress hypothesis,
    the results showed that children with difficult
    temperament (i.e., high activity level) were more
    vulnerable to family conflict and showed more
    delinquent symptoms as compared to children with
    relatively easy temperament (i.e., low activity
    level).
  • Moreover, as an important additional finding,
    children with difficult temperament were also
    influenced more positively by low conflict in
    their family than children with easy temperament.
    Difficult children showed fewer delinquent
    symptoms when reared in less conflictual
    families.

48
Discussion
  • Consistent with the diathesis-stress hypothesis,
    the results showed that children with difficult
    temperament (i.e., high activity level) were more
    vulnerable to family conflict and showed more
    delinquent symptoms as compared to children with
    relatively easy temperament (i.e., low activity
    level).
  • Moreover, as an important additional finding,
    children with difficult temperament were also
    influenced more positively by low conflict in
    their family than children with easy temperament.
    Difficult children showed fewer delinquent
    symptoms when reared in less conflictual
    families.
  • In sum, these findings were consistent with the
    differential susceptibility hypothesis (Belsky
    Pluess, 2009).

49
Discussion
  • Temperament Family Conflict interactions
    accounted for 5 of the variance in delinquent
    symptoms.
  • This effect size is generally consistent with
    results reported in other studies investigating
    Temperament Parenting interactions in the
    development of externalizing problems. Moreover,
    the interaction effects were independent of prior
    delinquent symptoms, maternal education, and the
    childs age and sex.

50
Discussion
  • Strengths
  • ---different informants significant
    interactions cannot be solely ascribed to shared
    method variances.
  • ---a longitudinal design was adopted
    inferences about direction of effects can be
    made.
  • ---the Temperament Environment interaction
    was first tested in a sample of Chinese children
    during the transition to nursery care.

51
Discussion
  • Limitations
  • ---sample characteristics attrition rates
    were moderate, and families from higher SES
    backgrounds were overrepresented, which may
    restrict the generalizability of the study.
  • ---measurement of temperament only three
    temperamental dimensions were measured in a
    single context with maternal reports.

52
Discussion
  • Moderation by gender?
  • Temperament-Environment correlation?

53
Conclusion
  • This study provides empirical evidence for the
    childrens differential susceptibility to family
    environments hypothesis using a sample of Chinese
    children during their transition to nursery care.
  • More specifically, our results confirmed the
    hypothesis that children with difficult
    temperament are more susceptible to family
    conflict, for better and for worse Compared with
    children with easy temperament, they showed more
    delinquent symptoms when exposed to family
    conflict, whereas they showed fewer symptoms in
    the absence of conflict, independent of maternal
    education and the childs age and sex.

54
Thank you!Wish you a pleasant time in Finland!
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com