Prospective Memory in Aviation, Everyday Tasks, and the Laboratory Key Dismukes Human Systems Integration Division NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field, CA Psychonomic Society 18 November 2006 - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 24
About This Presentation
Title:

Prospective Memory in Aviation, Everyday Tasks, and the Laboratory Key Dismukes Human Systems Integration Division NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field, CA Psychonomic Society 18 November 2006

Description:

Prospective Memory in Aviation, Everyday Tasks, and the Laboratory. Key Dismukes ... of next block increased to 8-12 sec and countdown clock appeared on screen ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:150
Avg rating:3.0/5.0

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Prospective Memory in Aviation, Everyday Tasks, and the Laboratory Key Dismukes Human Systems Integration Division NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field, CA Psychonomic Society 18 November 2006


1
Prospective Memory in Aviation, Everyday Tasks,
and the LaboratoryKey DismukesHuman Systems
Integration DivisionNASA Ames Research
CenterMoffett Field, CAPsychonomic Society18
November 2006

2
Major U.S. Airline Accidents Involving
Inadvertent Omission of a Normal Procedural Step
(1987-2001)
  • Five of 27 major accidents in which NTSB found
    crew error to be a causal factor
  • 1988, Detroit DC-9-82 Flaps/slats not set to
    takeoff position,154 killed 1 seriously injured
  • 1989, Dallas-Ft Worth B727 Flaps/slats not set
    to takeoff position, 14 killed 5 seriously
    injured
  • 1995, LaGuardia MD-82 Pitot-static heat not
    turned on, aircraft destroyed, no
    fatalities/serious injuries
  • 1997, Houston DC-9 Hydraulic boost pumps not set
    to high before landing, aircraft badly damaged,
    no fatalities/serious injuries
  • 2001, Little Rock MD-82 Spoilers not armed
    before landing, 11 killed 110 injured
  • Multiple factors involved, but a central aspect
    was experienced pilots forget to perform a
    normal, highly-practiced procedural step

3
Theoretical Accounts of Prospective Memory Still
in Infancy
  • Our view of evidence
  • After intention for deferred action is formed,
    attention turns to other tasks
  • Deferred intention fades from WM but remains in
    LTM
  • Retrieval requires noticing some cue associated
    in memory with intention
  • Can be environmental cue or stream-of-thought cue
  • Stored intention is retrieved if sufficient
    activation spreads to it from cue

4
Linking Real-World PM Phenomena to Underlying
Cognitive Processes
  • Three complementary approaches
  • Ethnographic
  • Analyses of accident/incident reports
  • Laboratory studies
  • Structure of airline operations well suited to
    study of skilled performance error
  • Operations are highly standardized
  • Can observe deviations from formal SOPs
  • Fair consensus about appropriate/inappropriate
    actions

5
An Ethnographic Study(Loukopoulos, Dismukes
Barshi, in preparation)
  • Focused on B737widely used in air transport
  • Reviewed written operating procedures,
    participated in classroom and flight simulation
    training at two major airlines
  • Observed large number of flights from cockpit
    jumpseat in normal revenue flights

6
Some Findings from Ethnographic Study
  • Pilots are
  • Frequently interrupted
  • Forced to postpone planned tasks
  • (Each) forced to perform multiple tasks in
    parallel
  • Timing and nature of task demands is not entirely
    predictable and is only partly under control of
    the crew
  • These results confirmed by a study analyzing NTSB
    reports of the 19 major U.S. airline accidents
    attributed primarily to crew error 1990-2001
    (Dismukes, Berman, Loukopoulos, in press)

7
Memory Errors Reported to the Aviation
Safety Reporting System (Nowinski, Holbrook,
Dismukes, 2003)
  • Sampled 20 of air carrier reports over 12-month
    period
  • Selected reports clearly involving some type of
    memory error
  • 74 of 75 reports involved prospective rather than
    retrospective memory
  • Does not necessarily indicate PM more common than
    RM failures, but suggests PM failures may be
  • more consequential
  • more likely to be reported
  • and/or more common

8
Varieties of Prospective Memory in the Cockpit
  • In these three studies analyzed
  • What tasks were being performed concurrently
  • Whether forgotten task was habitual
  • What cues would normally be present to trigger
    retrieval
  • Whether those cues were actually present
  • Concluded PM demands emerge in five types of
    situation
  • Episodic tasks
  • Habitual tasks
  • Atypical tasks substituted for habitual actions
  • Interrupted tasks
  • Interleaving tasks, including monitoring

9
(1) Episodic Tasks
Varieties of PM
  • Tasks not performed habitually
  • Type of PM task most often studied experimentally
  • Example ATC instructs crew to report passing
    through 10,000' during descent
  • In real world, ongoing tasks often divert
    attention from cues that might trigger retrieval
    (Holbrook, Dismukes Nowinski, 2005)
  • May be a major source of variance, but not
    studied extensively
  • This is the type of task studied most often in
    laboratory paradigms

10
(2) Habitual Tasks
Varieties of PM
  • Most cockpit tasks are habitual
  • Many tasks must be performed, many with multiple
    steps
  • Normally performed in set sequence by SOP
  • Execution becomes largely automaticdeliberate
    search of memory not required
  • Explicit (episodic) intention not
    requiredintention is implicit in action schema,
    stored as procedural memory
  • Performance of habitual tasks normally quite
    reliable
  • Performance undermined if normally present cues
    are removed
  • Example Setting flaps to takeoff position is
    deferred because of freezing slush on taxiway
  • Action out of sequence removes normal cues
    normal context
  • Habitual task becomes episodic task, but pilots
    may not realize need to encode explicit intention

11
(2) Habitual Tasks
Varieties of PM
  • Most cockpit tasks are habitual
  • Many tasks must be performed, many with multiple
    steps
  • Normally performed in set sequence by SOP
  • Execution becomes largely automaticdeliberate
    search of memory not required
  • Explicit (episodic) intention not
    requiredintention is implicit in action schema,
    stored as procedural memory
  • Performance of habitual tasks normally quite
    reliable
  • Performance undermined if normally present cues
    are removed
  • Example Setting flaps to takeoff position is
    deferred because of freezing slush on taxiway
  • Action out of sequence removes normal cues
    normal context
  • Habitual task becomes episodic task, but pilots
    may not realize need to encode explicit intention

12
(3) Atypical Actions Substituted for Habitual
Actions
Varieties of PM
  • Example Crew often departs from an airport via a
    Standard Instrument Departure requiring left turn
    to 300 degrees at 2000 feet
  • Execution of procedure becomes largely automatic
  • If ATC on one occasion amends instruction to turn
    left to 330 degrees at 2000 feet
  • Crew must form episodic intention to turn to 330
    and must inhibit normal response to level out at
    300 degrees
  • When busy with other tasks, crews vulnerable to
    habit intrusion (Reason, 1984) and level out
    prematurely at 300

13
(4) Interrupted Tasks
Varieties of PM
  • Interruptions very common in cockpit
  • Duration lasts from seconds to minutes
  • Interruptions are often so abrupt and salient
    that pilots may do little to encode explicit
    intention to resume interrupted task
  • Common error is to deal with interruption then
    proceed to next task, forgetting to complete
    interrupted task
  • Cockpit does not always provide salient cues for
    status of interrupted tasks
  • Perceptually rich cockpit environment associated
    with many tasks remaining to be performed

14
(5) Interleaving Tasks, Including Monitoring
Varieties of PM
  • While performing ongoing tasks pilots must
    periodically monitor status of other tasks, e.g.
  • While running checklists, communicating,
    programming, etc., First Officer must monitor
    progress of taxi by Captain
  • Monitoring is important, but difficult to
    maintain monitoring goal in WM as dual task if
    event rate low
  • Similar to difficulty with vigilance
    (Parasuraman, 1986) but different
  • Must interrupt ongoing task and shift attention
  • Attention allocated preferentially to areas with
    high info content over monitoring for low
    probability events, albeit with high consequences
    (see Wickens et al., 2003 for attention
    allocation model)
  • When attention shift delayed, monitoring task may
    slip from WM must be retrieved, as in other
    forms of PM

15
(5) Interleaving Tasks, Including Monitoring
Varieties of PM
  • While performing ongoing tasks pilots must
    periodically monitor status of other tasks, e.g.
  • While running checklists, communicating, etc.,
    First Officer must monitor progress of taxi by
    Captain
  • Monitoring is important, but difficult to
    maintain monitoring goal in WM as dual task if
    event rate low
  • Similar to difficulty with vigilance
    (Parasuraman, 1986) but different
  • Must interrupt ongoing task and shift attention
  • Attention allocated preferentially to areas with
    high info content over monitoring for low
    probability events, albeit with high consequences
    (see Wickens et al., 2003 for attention
    allocation model)
  • When attention shift delayed, monitoring task may
    slip from WM must be retrieved, as in other
    forms of PM

16
An Experimental Study of Interruptions(Dodhia
Dismukes, submitted)
  • From aviation studies, hypothesized individuals
    forget to resume interrupted tasks largely
    because
  • Salient intrusion of interruptions diverts
    attention, discouraging encoding explicit
    intention to resume or identifying specific
    reminder cues
  • Resumption will depend on happening to notice
    cues previously associated with interrupted
    taskvery unreliable
  • Cues indicating opportunity to resume interrupted
    task may not match form of (implicit or explicit)
    encoded intention
  • End of the interruption is not a discrete
    perceptual cue but a state of affairs requiring
    interpretation
  • Poor match between cues and encoding provides
    little activation to retrieve intention from LTM
  • End of interruption often followed by other task
    demands that divert attention from interpreting
    significance of completing interruption
  • New task goals spread activation to task subgoals
    rather than to retrieving implicit goal of
    resuming interrupted task

17
Experimental Paradigm
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
  • Participants answer questions resembling SAT,
    Category of questions changed between blocks
  • Instructed that when blocks were interrupted they
    should remember to resume interrupted block after
    completing interruption before starting new block
  • In baseline condition interruptions were
    abruptcurrent screen replaced with screen of
    different color and with different category of
    questions
  • After each completed block a screen appeared for
    2.5 sec Loading next block, followed by next
    block in series

18
Experimental Paradigm (continued)
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
  • Without explicit prompt participants had to
    remember to return to interrupted block
  • Proportion of successful resumptions of
    interrupted task in baseline condition 0.48

19
Encoding Manipulations
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
  • Hypothesis 1 Intrusion of sudden interruption
    discourages adequate encoding of explicit
    intention to resume interrupted task
  • Tested with encoding reminder manipulation
  • Interruption began with 4 sec message Please
    remember to return to the block that was just
    interrupted
  • Results Performance increased from 0.48 to 0.65
    (proportion of interrupted blocks resumed)
  • Was improvement due to reminder or to 4 sec delay
    before beginning interrupting task? Tested with
    encoding pause manipulation
  • Interruption began with 4 sec blank screen
  • Results Performance increased from 0.48 to 0.65
  • Conclusion Pause before beginning interrupting
    tasks helps remember to resume interrupted task

20
Retrieval Manipulations
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
  • Hypothesis 2 Individuals are likely to forget to
    resume interrupted tasks because of mismatch
    between form of encoded intention and diverse
    cues that must be interpreted to recognize
    interruption has ended
  • Tested with retrieval reminder manipulation
  • 2.5 sec Loading next block screen also said
    End of interruption
  • Results Performance increased from 0.48 to 0.90
    (proportion of interrupted blocks resumed)
  • Hypothesis 3 Individuals are likely to forget to
    resume interrupted tasks if interrupting task is
    directly followed by other tasks that demand
    attention
  • Tested with retrieval pause manipulation
  • Interval between end of interruption and
    beginning of next block increased to 8-12 sec and
    countdown clock appeared on screen
  • Results Performance increased from 0.52 to 0.88
    (proportion of interrupted blocks resumed)

21
Summary of Results
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
22
Implications
An Experimental Study of Interruptions
  • Theoretical
  • Supports three hypotheses but more research
    needed
  • Consistent with model of PM in which intentions
    are retrieved from LTM by activation from cues
    processed attentively
  • Practical
  • Pausing before dealing with interruptions to
    explicitly encode intention to resume interrupted
    task and to identify specific reminder cues
  • Periodically pausing after completing tasks to
    ask what other tasks remain to be done

23
Final Thoughts
  • Prospective memory probably operates in similar
    ways in the skilled tasks performed by other
    kinds of experts, e.g., medical personnel
  • To be effective, countermeasures to PM errors
    must be realistic for experts to apply while
    performing their task
  • Understanding the real-world performance requires
    integrating diverse research approaches,
    including observing real-world performance and
    well-controlled experimental studies
  • This integrative approach enhances the power of
    both field and lab studies

24
  • Thanks to my colleagues in this research
  • Rahul Dodhia, Jon Holbrook, Kim Jobe,
  • Loukia Loukopoulos, and Jessica Nowinski
  • For more information
  • http//human-factors.arc.nasa.gov/ihs/flightcognit
    ion
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com