ERP responses to violations of morphological structure Joanna Morris Florack1 - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 2
About This Presentation
Title:

ERP responses to violations of morphological structure Joanna Morris Florack1

Description:

If morphologically complex words are represented in decomposed form, they must ... Psychophysiology, 30(1), 47-61. McKinnon, R., Allen, M., & Osterhout, L. (2003) ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:61
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 3
Provided by: UGAP8
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: ERP responses to violations of morphological structure Joanna Morris Florack1


1
ERP responses to violations of morphological
structureJoanna Morris Florack1 Phillip J.
Holcomb2Hampshire College, Amherst, MA1 Tufts
University, Medford, MA2
  • Introduction
  • If morphologically complex words are represented
    in decomposed form, they must be recombined at
    some stage of processing.
  • If combinatorial processes play a role at both
    the morphological and syntactic levels of
    linguistic representation, it may be that the
    cognitive processes responsible for the
    combination of both morphological and syntactic
    units share a single underlying brain mechanism.
  • If so, we would expect that violations of
    morphological structure to give rise to
    components similar to those seen in response to
    violations of syntactic structure.
  • We can then use the presence of absence of
    components such as the ELAN, LAN and P600 that
    have been shown to be associated with syntactic
    processing to distinguish between whole-word and
    decompositional accounts of morphological
    representation and processing.

Figure 5 Experiment 2Semantic Categorization
Task (Subjects with no Anterior Negativity)
Figure 3 Experiment 1Grammaticality Judgment
Task
For experiment 2 , we divided subjects into two
groups on the basis of their responses to the
morphological violations in the 300-500 ms time
window. Data for 8 subjects who did not show an
anterior negativity were analysed separately.
  • Methods
  • Experiment 1Grammaticality Judgment Task
  • 16 participants (12 female, mean age 21.7
    years)
  • 80 regular and irregular verbs matched for
    frequency and length, divided into 2 lists
  • Each verb appeared in infinitive and in past
    tense form irregular verbs also appeared in a
    morphologically incorrect (regularized) form,
    e.g. bringed
  • 120 fillers--40 additional incorrectly inflected
    irregular verbs and 80 non-word fillers.
  • Subjects were instructed to a button if the word
    was ungrammatical or wrong, i.e. it was not a
    normal English word, and another if the word was
    correct.
  • Experiment 2Semantic Categorization Task
  • 22 participants (15 female, mean age 22.2
    years)
  • Stimuli were the same as in experiment 1.
  • Fourteen probe verbs were also included in each
    block.
  • All probes were members of one of five semantic
    categoriesFOOD, EMOTION, VIOLENCE, SPORT or BED.
    Each block was preceded by one of these category
    names which signaled to the subject that their
    task was to monitor the list of words for words
    related to that category and press a button on a
    game controller when such a word appeared.

2
Figure 1 Procedure
  • Discussion
  • The presence of a late posterior positivity in
    response to morphological violations in a single
    word paradigm suggests that late positivities,
    such as the P600, may reflect problems with
    combinatorial processes at multiple levels of
    linguistic analysis, both syntactic and
    morphological.
  • In addition these data provide evidence for the
    notion that the specific processing strategies
    and linguistic competences that subjects bring
    with them determine, in part, both the category
    some particular event falls into and the brain
    response elicited by that event. (Osterhout,
    1997 p. 514). In the grammaticality judgment
    task, in which the syntactic properties of words
    were relevant to the task, all subjects showed a
    posterior positivity for the morphological
    violations. In the semantic categorization study,
    where syntactic properties were not task
    relevant, some subjects were sensitive to the
    morphological composition of the regularized
    irregulars other subjects seemed to treat these
    words as morphologically simple pseudo-words as
    evidenced by the presence of a negativity in the
    400-500 ms time window. These findings are
    consistent with others recently reported by
    McKinnon, Allen and Osterhout (2003) who showed
    that bound stem non-words elicit a brain response
    similar to that elicited by bound stem real
    words, indicating that these words may be
    morphologically decomposed. Our data expand upon
    these findings by showing that these effects are
    sensitive to individual differences.
  • Although it is unclear how to best categorize the
    anterior negativities we observed, their presence
    for irregular past tenses in Experiment 1 is
    consistent with a view of the morphological
    processes in which the past tense forms of
    regular verbs are generated by rule while
    irregular forms are stored in memory (Pinker,
    1997). If irregular past tenses are retrieved as
    whole forms from the lexicon, we would not expect
    to see components that index syntactic
    operations, but rather, those that are indicative
    of lexical access and/or semantic integration,
    such as the N400 (Holcomb,1993) or the LAN
    (Friederici, 1995). Our data are consistent with
    this latter view.

An example of a two-trial sequence at the
beginning of a block for the semantic
categorization task. (Probe items are indicated
in red). The procedure for the grammaticality
judgment task was identical, with the exception
that there were no category indicators at the
beginning of the block and no probe items.
Dashes enclosed by parentheses are blink stimuli.
Figure 4 Experiment 2Semantic Categorization
Task (All Subjects)
Figure 2 Electrode Montage
References Friederici, A. D. (1995). The time
course of syntactic activation during language
processing A model based on neuropsychological
and neurophysiological data. Brain Language,
50(3), 259-281. Holcomb, P. J. (1993). Semantic
priming and stimulus degradation Implications
for the role of the N400 in language processing.
Psychophysiology, 30(1), 47-61. McKinnon, R.,
Allen, M., Osterhout, L. (2003). Morphological
decomposition involving non-productive morphemes
ERP evidence. Neuroreport, 14(6),
883-886. Osterhout, L. (1997). On the brain
response to syntactic anomalies Manipulations of
word position and word class reveal individual
differences. Brain Language, 59(3),
494-522. Pinker, S. (1997). Words and rules in
the human brain. Nature, 387(6633), 547-548.
This research was supported by HD25889 and
HD043251
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com