Gender Typicality in Childrens Spoken Narratives: Acoustic and Perceptual Analyses Students: Heather - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

Gender Typicality in Childrens Spoken Narratives: Acoustic and Perceptual Analyses Students: Heather

Description:

Department of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, College of Liberal Arts ... Adult men and women pronounce speech sounds differently from one another. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:40
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: urop2
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Gender Typicality in Childrens Spoken Narratives: Acoustic and Perceptual Analyses Students: Heather


1
Gender Typicality in Childrens Spoken
NarrativesAcoustic and Perceptual
AnalysesStudents Heather Bauer, Alysse Zittnan,
Adviser Benjamin MunsonDepartment of
Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, College of
Liberal Arts
  • Gender Differences in Adults Speech
  • Spoken language is highly variable. Variability
    affects all aspects of language sentence
    structures, discourse strategies, word choice,
    and pronunciation.
  • Adult men and women pronounce speech sounds
    differently from one another. These differences
    are evident even in short stretches of speech,
    and allow a talkers sex to be ascertained at
    greater-than-chance levels even when other
    aspects of linguistic variation are controlled
  • The differences are not solely the consequence of
    anatomic and physiologic differences between the
    sexes. We know this because
  • 1. They are language- and culture-specific (Van
    Bejooijen, 1995)
  • 2. There is only a weak correlation between
    variation in pronunciation and variation in the
    morphology of the speech-production mechanism
  • 3. They are not across the board (Munson et
    al., 2006). They are more evident in some sound
    classes than in others. Moreover, there are
  • Gender Differences in Childrens Speech
  • Adults perceive boys and girls to sound different
    from one another well in advance of the sex
    differentiation in the speech-production
    mechanism that occurs at puberty (Perry, Ohde,
    Ashmead, 2001 inter alia).
  • Gender differences in childrens pronunciation
    are not across the board. As with adults, boys
    and girls speech differs more in some sounds and
    sound classes than others.
  • Ergo, sex differences are to represent learned
    behaviors that are the result of selective
    attention to and emulation of specific models in
    the ambient language.
  • In our laboratory, we have studied gender
    typicality in childrens speech by comparing the
    productions of 5-13 year old boys with GID (i.e.,
    boys whose gender expression has been deemed not
    to meet cultural expectations) to children whose
    gender expression is deemed to meet cultural
    expectations (henceforth Expected Gender
    Development EGD). This provides us with an
    opportunity to study gender in speech without the
    confounding influence of biological sex.
  • Results Acoustic Analyses
  • When the average acoustic characteristics of
    vowels were considered, the only difference to
    approach statistical significance was one measure
    of formant frequencies of vowels. There were no
    differences for the consonants s or sh
  • Regression analyses predicting perceived gender
    typicality from acoustic variables found that
    adult listeners paid particular attention to the
    way the vowel /æ/ was spoken. There was a weak
    association between the acoustic characteristics
    of s and sh. This is consistent with
    previous work on adults, in which it has been
    shown that adult attend to the characteristics of
    these sounds when judging the gender typicality
    of adults' voices. Together, 41 of variance in
    the ratings was accounted for.
  • Speech Samples
  • The recorded spoken narratives used in this
    experiment were elicited by asking each child to
    tell a story about a picture (see title banner).
    This was taken from a standardized test of
    childrens narrative abilities. Narratives were
    elicited as part of a larger data-collection
    protocol that included measures of single-word
    productions and sentence repetitions.
  • Data from 28 children was used. Each speaker was
    categorized as either younger (average age 7
    years) or older (average age 10 years) and as
    either GID or EGD. The diagnoses of GID were
    made by psychometrists with expertise in
    childrens gender development. We used seven
    speakers from each category for a total of 28
    narratives, varying in length from about 30
    seconds to 1 minute.
  • Results Perception Task
  • Two, two-factor within-subjects ANOVAs examined
    the influence of gender typicality (GID vs. EGD)
    and age group on perceived gender and perceived
    age.
  • For perceived gender, there was a significant
    main effect of gender group, F1,24 118.2, p lt
    0.001, partial h2 0.83. This interacted
    significantly with age, F1,24 4.6, p 0.042,
    partial h2 0.16. Figure 1 shows that the
    interaction occurred because the older GID boys
    sounded less boy-like than the younger GID boys,
    while the older EGD boys sounded more boy-like
    than the younger EGD boys.
  • For perceived age, there was a significant main
    effect of both gender group (F1,24 19.2, p lt
    0.001, partial h2 0.44) and age group (F1,24
    178.4, p lt 0.001, partial h2 0.88). These
    interacted significantly, F1,24 24.0, p lt
    0.001, partial h2 0.50. Figure 2 shows that
    the interaction occurred because the older GID
    boys were rated to sound older than the older EGD
    boys, while there were no differences for the
    younger group.
  • Acoustic Measures
  • The acoustic analysis was conducted with the
    Praat signal-processing program.
  • Our analysis focused on acoustic correlates of
    the articulation of vowels and consonants,
    including vowels fundamental frequency (the
    acoustic parameter that is perceived as pitch),
    vowels formant frequencies (the acoustic
    parameters that are perceived as vowel quality),
    and two consonants known to relate to the
    perception of gender, s and sh. These
    parameters were chosen based on previous studies
    that compared speech differences of adult men and
    women (e.g., Munson et al., 2006).
  • Inter-rater reliability was assessed by having
    both students analyze randomly selected passages
    independently and compare their measures.

.
Figure 6. Perceived Gender based on Single
Words, Sentences, and Narratives
Figures 1 and 2. Spectrograms of the word sun
produced by a Boy with EGD (left) and
GID (right). The green circle is the s sound
and the red square is the first two formant
frequencies for the u sound.
  • A comparison with previous acoustic and
    perceptual analyses of these childrens single
    word and sentence productions data showed that
    the ratings of the gender typicality of the
    narratives better differentiated between the two
    groups than the ratings gender from words or
    sentences
  • However, the correlation between acoustic
    measures and the perceptual measures was weaker
    for the narratives than it was for the single
    words or the sentences.
  • Listeners might attended to suprasegmental
    features (i.e., intonation, rate of speech) or
    narrative content more than pronunciation
  • Discussion
  • As expected, children with GID were rated as
    sounding less boy-like than their EGD peers.
    This tendency was stronger in the groups of older
    (10 year old) boys than in the younger (7 year
    old) boys. In fact, the older boys with GID were
    rated to sound less boy-like than the younger
    boys.
  • When compared with previous perceptual data,
    stronger differentiation between groups was found
    for the narratives than for the single word
    productions or the sentences. This suggests that
    the less-structured task of narrative production
    gives children more opportunities to use
    permissible linguistic variation to convey gender
  • At the same time, the fact that there was a
    smaller correlation between acoustic measures and
    perceptual measures for narratives than for
    single words and sentences suggests that some of
    the parameters that adults use to perceive gender
    in childrens speech are not related to
    pronunciation.
  • Our ongoing research is examining the extent to
    which gender-typicality judgments are related to
    judgments of childrens overall language ability.
    Our perceived age findings suggests a positive
    relationship between how boy-like a child sounds
    and how developmentally advanced they are.
  • Perception Task
  • Twenty-five adult participants were asked to
    listen to each narrative and provide a rating of
    gender on a 6-point scale, where 6 indicated
    "definitely a boy", 1 indicated "definitely a
    girl", and 2 through 5 indicated various degrees
    of uncertainty (see Figure 3). They also
    provided an estimate of the child's age in years.
    The order of presentation was fully randomized.
    The task took place in a sound-treated booth.
    Responses were logged using the E-Prime software.
  • Listeners were unaware that they were listening
    to children with GID and children with EGD. They
    were also not told that all of the children were
    boys.
  • Purpose
  • To examine whether the spoken narratives of
    children with GID differ from those of children
    with EGD perceptually (i.e., do adults rate the
    children as sounding different?) and acoustically
    (i.e., can we find evidence that children are
    producing speech differently?)
  • Compare the acoustic and perceptual measures to
    previously made acoustic and perceptual measures
    of these childrens productions of single words
    and sentences, to examine the influence of
    linguistic complexity on the expression and
    perception of gender in childrens speech
  • Significance
  • Childrens gender expression is evidence of
    selective attention during language acquisition.
    Most theories of language posit that children
    treat the input uniformly. The results of this
    research have the potential to question this
    assumption, and suggests that models of language
    acquisition must posit a stronger role for
    selective attention during acquisition.

Figure 3. Perceived Gender Typicality
Figure 4. Perceived Age
Acknowledgements The research on the acoustic and
perceptual characteristics of the speech of
children with Gender Identity Disorder was
completed in collaboration initiated by J.
Michael Bailey of the Northwestern University
Department of Psychology. The funding for the
data collection phase of this project was
provided by a grant to Janet Pierrehumbert
(Department of Linguistics, Northwestern
University) and J. Michael Bailey from the
Northwestern University Office of the Vice
President for Research. The data-collection
protocol was designed by Janet Pierrehumbert,
Benjamin Munson, and Karla K. McGregor
(Department of Speech-Language Pathology and
Audiology, University of Iowa). Data collection
was supervised by Ken Zucker (Clark Institute for
Mental Heath, Toronto, Ontario). The acoustic
analysis protocol was developed by Benjamin
Munson and Janet Pierrehumbert. The perceptual
analysis protocol was developed by Benjamin
Munson. All of the acoustic and perceptual
analyses described herein were conducted at the
University of Minnesota. The authors of this
poster take sole responsibility for the
interpretations that are presented herein, and
acknowledges that the different collaborators on
this project might interpret these same findings
differently. References Munson, B., Jefferson,
S.V. McDonald, E.C. (2006). The influence of
perceived sexual orientation on fricative
perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 119, 2427-2437 Perry, T.L., Ohde, R.,
Ashmead, D. (2001). The acoustic bases for
gender identification from children's voices.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
109, 2988-2998. Van Bezooijen, R. (1995).
Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences
between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and
Speech, 38, 253-265.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com