Preventing and Quitting Substance abuse

1 / 59
About This Presentation
Title:

Preventing and Quitting Substance abuse

Description:

Preventing and Quitting Substance abuse Smoking DiClemente et al 1991 A study to examine the stages of change in predicting smoking cessation DiClemente and Prochaska ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:70
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 60
Provided by: homepageN8

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Preventing and Quitting Substance abuse


1
Preventing and Quitting Substance abuse
  • Smoking

2
DiClemente et al 1991
  • A study to examine the stages of change in
    predicting smoking cessation
  • DiClemente and Prochaska (1982) developed their
    transtheoretical model of change to examine the
    stages of change in addictive behaviours. This
    study examined the validity of the stages of
    change model and assessed the relationship
    between stage of change and smoking cessation.

3
The stages of change model
  • The stages of change model describes the
    following stages
  • Precontemplation not seriously considering
    quitting in the next 6 months.
  • Contemplation considering quitting in the next
    6 months.
  • Action making behavioural changes.
  • Maintenance maintaining these changes.

4
The stages of change model
  • Subjects
  • 1466 subjects were recruited for a minimum
    intervention smoking cessation programme from
    Texas and Rhode Island. The majority of the
    subjects were white, female, started smoking at
    about 16 and smoked on average 29 cigarettes a
    day.

5
The stages of change model
  • Design The subjects completed a set of measures
    at baseline and were followed up at 1 and 6
    months

6
The stages of change model
  • Measures The subjects completed the following set
    of measures
  • Smoking abstinence self-efficacy (DiClemente et
    al. 1985), which measures a smoker's confidence
    that they will not smoke in 20 challenging
    situations.

7
The stages of change model
  • Perceived stress scale (Cohen et al. 1985),
    which measures how much perceived stress an
    individual has experienced in the past month.
  • Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire, which
    measures physical tolerance to nicotine.

8
The stages of change model
  • Smoking decisional balance scale (Velicer et
    al. 1985), which measures the perceived pros and
    cons of smoking.

9
The stages of change model
  • Smoking processes of change scale (DiClemente
    and Prochaska 1985), which measures an
    individual's stage of change. According to this
    scale, the subjects were defined as
    precontemplators (n 166), contemplators (n
    794) or as being in the preparation stage (n
    506).
  • Demographic data, including age, gender,
    education and smoking history.

10
The stages of change model
  • At baseline the results showed that those in the
    preparation stage smoked less, were less
    addicted, had higher self-efficacy, rated the
    pros of smoking as less and the costs of smoking
    as more, and had attempted to quit more often
    than the other two groups. At both 1 and 6
    months, the subjects in the preparation stage had
    attempted to quit more often and were more likely
    not to be smoking.

11
Interventions to promote cessation
  • Interventions to promote cessation can be
    described as
  • (1) clinical interventions, which are aimed at
    the individual,
  • (2) self-help movements
  • (3) public health interventions, which are aimed
    at populations.

12
Clinical interventions promoting individual
change
  • Disease perspectives on cessation
  • Nicotine fading procedures encourage smokers
    gradually to switch to brands of low nicotine
    cigarettes and gradually to smoke fewer
    cigarettes.

13
Clinical interventions promoting individual
change
  • It is believed that when the smoker is ready to
    quit completely, their addiction to nicotine will
    be small enough to minimise any withdrawal
    symptoms. Although there is no evidence to
    support the effectiveness of nicotine fading on
    its own, it has been shown to be useful alongside
    other methods such as relapse prevention (e.g.
    Brown et al. 1984). But other evidence shows that
    people compensate by smoking more low-nicotine
    cigarettes.

14
Nicotine replacement.
  • For example, nicotine chewing gum. The chewing
    gum has been shown to be a useful addition to
    other behavioural methods, particularly in
    preventing short-term relapse (Killen et al.
    1990). However, it tastes unpleasant and takes
    time to be absorbed into the bloodstream.

15
Nicotine replacement.
  • More recently, nicotine patches have become
    available and only need to be applied once a day
    in order to provide a steady supply of nicotine
    into the bloodstream. They do not need to be
    tasted, although it could be argued that chewing
    gum satisfies the oral component of smoking.

16
Nicotine replacement.
  • However, whether nicotine replacement procedures
    are actually compensating for a physiological
    addiction or whether they are offering a placebo
    effect via expecting not to need cigarettes is
    unclear.

17
Nicotine replacement.
  • Smokers are not a homogenous group. Some smokers
    may smoke predominantly out of habit some due to
    an addiction to nicotine (Fagerstrom 1982).
    Accordingly, the same therapeutic approach may
    not be optimal for both groups.

18
Nicotine replacement.
  • Indeed, there is evidence that cognitive-behaviour
    al approaches may be best for those who smoke
    predominantly out of habit, while nicotine
    replacements in combination with some form of
    psychological intervention may prove optimal for
    those with high levels of nicotine dependency.

19
Nicotine replacement.
  • Evidence in support of this hypothesis was
    provided by Hall et at. (1985), who assigned high
    and low nicotine-dependent smokers to either an
    intensive behavioural intervention, nicotine gum,
    or a combination of both approaches. At the
    one-year follow-up, 50 per cent of high
    nicotine-dependent smokers in the combined
    intervention were not smoking.

20
Nicotine replacement.
  • This compared with abstinence rates of 28 per
    cent among the equivalent group in the nicotine
    gum condition, and 11 per cent of those who
    participated in behavioural intervention. In
    contrast, low dependent smokers gained most from
    the behavioural intervention. Among this group,
    abstinence rates at one year were 47 per cent, in
    comparison to rates of 42 and 38 per cent in the
    nicotine gum and combined interventions.

21
Social learning perspectives on cessation
  • 1 Aversion therapies
  • aim to punish smoking rather than reward it.
    Early methodologies used crude techniques such as
    electric shocks, whereby each time an individual
    puffed on a cigarette or drank some alcohol they
    received a mild electric shock. However, this
    approach was found to be ineffective for smoking
    and drinking (e.g. Wilson 1978), the main reason
    being that it is difficult to transfer
    behaviours, which have been learnt in the
    laboratory to the real world.

22
Army aversion therapy for homosexuality
23
Rapid smoking
  • Rapid smoking is a more successful form of
    aversion therapy (Danaher 1977) and aims to make
    the actual process of smoking unpleasant. Smokers
    are required to sit in a closed room and take a
    puff every 6 seconds until it becomes so
    unpleasant they can't smoke anymore. Although
    there is some evidence to support rapid smoking
    as a smoking cessation technique, it has obvious
    side-effects, including increased blood carbon
    monoxide levels and heart rates.

24
focused smoking
  • Other aversion therapies include focused smoking,
    which involves smokers concentrating on all the
    negative experiences of smoking, and smoke
    holding, which involves smokers holding smoke in
    their mouths for a period of time and again
    thinking about the unpleasant sensations. Smoke
    holding has been shown to be more successful at
    promoting cessation than focused smoking and it
    doesn't have the side-effects of rapid smoking
    (Walker and Franzini 1985).

25
Contingency contracting.
  • Schwartz (1987) analysed a series of contingency
    contracting studies for smoking cessation that
    took place between 1967 and 1985 and concluded
    that this procedure seems to be successful in
    promoting initial cessation, but once the
    contract is finished, or the money returned,
    relapse is common.

26
Cue exposure procedures
  • Cue exposure procedures focus on the
    environmental factors that have become associated
    with smoking and drinking. For example, if an
    individual always smokes when they drink alcohol,
    alcohol will become a strong external cue to
    smoke and vice versa. Cue exposure techniques
    gradually expose the individual to different cues
    and encourage them to develop coping strategies
    to deal with them. This procedure aims to
    extinguish the response to the cues over time and
    is opposite to cue avoidance procedures, which
    encourage individuals not to go to the places
    where they may feel the urge to smoke.

27
Self-management procedures
  • Self-management procedures use a variety of
    behavioural techniques to promote smoking and
    drinking cessation in individuals and may be
    carried out under professional guidance. Such
    procedures involve self monitoring (keeping a
    record of own smoking/drinking behaviour),
    becoming aware of the causes of smoking/drinking
    (What makes me smoke? Where do I smoke? Where do
    I drink?), and becoming aware of the consequences
    of smoking /drinking (Does it make me feel
    better? What do I expect from smoking/drinking?).
    However, used on their own self-management
    techniques do not appear to be any more
    successful than other interventions (Hall et al.
    1990).

28
Self-help movements
  • Although clinical and public health interventions
    have proliferated over the last few decades, up
    to 90 per cent of ex-smokers report having
    stopped without any formal help (Fiore et al.
    1990). Lichtenstein and Glasgow (1992) reviewed
    the literature on self-help quitting and reported
    that success rates tend to be about 10-20 per
    cent at 1-year follow-up and 3-5 per cent for
    continued cessation.

29
Self-help movements
  • The literature suggests that lighter smokers are
    more likely to be successful at self quitting
    than heavy smokers and that minimal interventions
    such as follow-up telephone calls can improve the
    rate of success. However, although many
    ex-smokers report that 'I did it on my own', it
    is important not to discount their exposure to
    the multitude of health education messages
    received via television, radio or leaflets.

30
Public health interventions
  • promoting cessation among populations

31
Doctor's advice.
  • In a classic study carried out in five general
    practices in London (Russell et al. 1979),
    smokers visiting their GP over a 4-week period
    were allocated to one of four groups
  • (1) follow-up only,
  • (2) questionnaire about their smoking behaviour
    and follow-up,
  • (3) doctor's advice to stop smoking,
    questionnaire about their smoking behaviour and
    follow-up,
  • (4) doctor's advice to stop smoking, leaflet
    giving tips on how to stop and follow-up.

32
Results at 12 months
33
Worksite interventions.
  • Research into the effectiveness of no-smoking
    policies has produced conflicting results, with
    some studies reporting an overall reduction in
    the number of cigarettes smoked for up to 12
    months (e.g. Biener et al. 1989) and others
    suggesting that smoking outside work hours
    compensates for any reduced smoking in the
    workplace (e.g. Gomel et al. 1993). In two
    Australian studies, public service workers were
    surveyed about their attitudes to smoking bans in
    44 government office buildings immediately after
    the ban and 6 months later.

34
Worksite interventions.
  • The results suggested that although immediately
    after the ban many smokers felt inconvenienced,
    these attitudes improved at 6 months with both
    smokers and non-smokers recognizing the benefits
    of the ban. However, only 2 per cent stopped
    smoking during this period.

35
A pilot study to examine the effects of a
workplace ban on smoking on craving, stress and
other behaviours (Gomel et al. 1993)
  • The ban was introduced on 1 August 1989 at the
    New South Wales Ambulance Service in Australia.
    This study is interesting because it included
    physiological measures of smoking to identify any
    compensatory smoking.

36
Subjects
  • A screening question showed that 60 per cent (n
    47) of the employees were currently smoking.
    Twenty-four subjects (15 males and 9 females)
    completed all measures. They had an average age
    of 34 years, had smoked on average for 11 years
    and smoked on average 26 cigarettes a day.

37
Design
  • The subjects completed a set of measures 1 week
    before the ban (time 1) and 1 (time 2) and 6
    weeks (time 3) after.

38
Measures
  • At times 1, 2 and 3, the subjects were evaluated
    for cigarette and alcohol consumption,
    demographic information (e.g. age), exhaled
    carbon monoxide and blood cotinine (The major
    metabolite of nicotine that indicates levels of
    nicotine intake). The subjects also completed
    daily record cards for 5 working days and 2
    non-working days, including measures of smoking,
    alcohol consumption, snack intake and ratings of
    subjective discomfort.

39
The results
  • The results showed a reduction in self-reports of
    smoking in terms of number of cigarettes smoked
    during a working day and the number smoked during
    working hours at both the 1-week and 6-week
    follow-ups compared with baseline, indicating
    that the smokers were smoking less following the
    ban. However, although there was an initial
    reduction in nicotine at week 1, by 6 weeks blood
    nicotine levels were almost back to baseline
    levels, suggesting that the smokers may have been
    compensating for the ban by smoking more outside
    the workplace.

40
The results
  • The results also showed reductions in craving and
    stress following the ban these lower levels of
    stress were maintained, whereas craving gradually
    returned to baseline (supporting compensatory
    smoking). The results showed no increases in
    snack intake or alcohol consumption.

41
Comment
  • The self-report data from this study suggest that
    worksite bans may be an effective form of public
    health intervention for reducing smoking.
    However, the physiological data suggest that
    simply introducing a no smoking policy may not be
    sufficient, as smokers may show compensatory
    smoking.

42
Government interventions.
  • Restrictinglbanning advertising.
  • Increasing the cost. Research indicates a
    relationship between the cost of cigarettes and
    alcohol and their consumption.

43
Government interventions.
  • Banning smoking in public places. Smoking is
    already restricted to specific places in many
    countries (e.g. in the UK most public transport
    is no smoking). A wider ban on smoking may
    promote smoking-cessation. According to social
    learning theory, this would result in the cues to
    smoking (e.g. restaurants, bars) becoming
    eventually disassociated from smoking. However,
    it is possible that this would simply result in
    compensatory smoking in other places.

44
Government interventions.
  • Banning cigarette smoking and alcohol drinking.
    But the government loses tax and consumption is
    driven underground, just as drug-taking is. Also
    consider the unsuccessful prohibition era in the
    USA.

45
Methodological problems evaluating clinical and
public health interventions
  • Who has become a non-smoker? Someone who hasn't
    smoked in the last month/week/day? Someone who
    regards themselves as a non-smoker? (Smokers are
    notorious for under-reporting their smoking.)
    Does a puff of a cigarette count as smoking? Do
    cigars count as smoking? These questions need to
    be answered to assess success rates.

46
Methodological problems evaluating clinical and
public health interventions
  • Should non-smokers be believed when they say they
    don't smoke? Methods other than self-report exist
    to assess smoking behaviour, such as carbon
    monoxide in the breath, cotinine in the saliva.
    These are more accurate but are time-consuming
    and expensive.

47
Methodological problems evaluating clinical and
public health interventions
  • How should smokers be assigned to different
    interventions? For success rates to be
    calculated, comparisons need to be made between
    different types of intervention (e.g. aversion
    therapy vs cue exposure). These groups should
    obviously be matched for age, gender, ethnicity
    and smoking behaviour. Subjects could be matched
    on what stage of change (contemplation vs
    precontemptation vs preparation) they are at, or
    on health beliefs such as self-efficacy, or costs
    and benefits of smoking. The list of items to
    match on is endless, but it is difficult to find
    subjects that match if many variables to match on
    are used.

48
relapse rates
  • Although many people are successful at initially
    stopping smoking and changing their drinking
    behaviour, relapse rates are high. Interestingly,
    the pattern for relapse is consistent across a
    number of different addictive behaviours, with
    high rates initially tapering off over a year.

49
(No Transcript)
50
relapse prevention model of addictions
  • Marlatt and Gordon (1985) developed a relapse
    prevention model of addictions which specifically
    examined the processes involved in successful and
    unsuccessful cessation attempts. The relapse
    prevention model was based on the following
    concept of addictive behaviours

51
relapse prevention model of addictions
  • Addictive behaviours are learned and therefore
    can be unlearned they are reversible.
  • Addictions are not 'all or nothing' but exist
    on a continuum.
  • Lapses from abstinence are likely and
    acceptable.
  • Believing that 'one drink-a drunk' is a
    self-fulfilling prophecy.

52
relapse prevention model of addictions
  • They distinguished between a lapse, which entails
    a minor slip (e.g. a cigarette, a couple of
    drinks), and a relapse, which entails a return to
    former behaviour (e.g. smoking 20 cigarettes,
    getting drunk). Marlatt and Gordon examined the
    processes involved in the progression from
    abstinence to relapse and in particular assessed
    the mechanisms which may explain the transition
    from lapse to relapse

53
(No Transcript)
54
(No Transcript)
55
Aids are not being used
  • Nearly 80 per cent of smokers who attempt to
    quit, do so without using any method
    ofassistance, like nicotine replacement therapy
    orcounselling. Yet research shows that smokers
    who use aids are twice as likely to achieve
    long-term abstinence from smoking. David
    Hammond(University of Waterloo, Ontario) and
    colleagues interviewed 616 adult smokers over the
    telephone to investigate why so few smokers use
    available aids to help them stop smoking.

56
Aids are not being used
  • It could be because they don't know about the
    available aids. Indeed, when asked to"list as
    many different methods...for quitting smoking as
    you can", a third ofHammond's sample failed to
    mention nicotine replacement therapies, and only
    halfmentioned Zyban, an antidepressant known to
    help people quit. At the same time, a quarter of
    participants mentioned an aid to stoppingsmoking
    for which there's no evidence of effectiveness,
    like hypnosis or acupuncture.

57
Aids are not being used
  • There was also evidence that smokers don't
    believe available aids are effective.Nearly 80
    per cent said they thought they would be as
    successful quitting on their own, as with help.

58
Aids are not being used
  • And failing to recognise the effectiveness of
    'quitting aids' could be underminingsmokers'
    attempts to stop. Three months after the initial
    telephone survey, thoseparticipants who had
    rated 'quitting aids' as effective, were twice as
    likely to havesince made an attempt to quit.

59
The End
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)