Children's Expression of Emotional Meaning in Music Through Expressive Movement - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 26
About This Presentation
Title:

Children's Expression of Emotional Meaning in Music Through Expressive Movement

Description:

Children's Expression of Emotional Meaning in Music Through Expressive Movement – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:351
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 27
Provided by: rthoma
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Children's Expression of Emotional Meaning in Music Through Expressive Movement


1
Children's Expression of Emotional Meaning in
Music Through Expressive Movement
  • R. Thomas Boone
  • Assumption College
  • Brandeis University
  • Joseph G. Cunningham
  • Brandeis University

2
Abstract
  • Recent research has demonstrated that preschool
    children can decode emotional meaning in
    expressive body movements however, to date, no
    research has considered preschool childrens
    ability to encode emotional meaning in gestures
    and expressive movement. The current study
    investigated 4- (N23) and 5- (N24) year-old
    childrens ability to encode the emotional
    meaning of an accompanying music segment by
    moving a teddy bear using previously modeled
    gestures and expressive movement to indicate one
    of four target emotions (happiness, sadness,
    anger, or fear). Adult judges visually
    categorized the silent videotaped gestural and
    expressive movement performances by the children
    ages with greater than chance level accuracy. In
    addition, accuracy in categorizing the emotion
    being expressed varied as a function of age of
    child and emotion. A subsequent cue analysis
    revealed that children as young as 4-years-old
    were systematically varying their gestural
    movements with respect to force, rotation, shifts
    in movement pattern, tempo, and upward movement
    in the process of emotional communication. The
    theoretical significance of such encoding ability
    is discussed with respect to childrens nonverbal
    skills and the communication of emotion.

3
Introduction
  • Childrens Emotional Understanding of Music
  • - The intrinsic structural specification
    hypothesis argues that higher-order invariants of
    musical structure intrinsically specify some
    forms of emotional meaning in music(Clynes, 1982
    Cunningham Sterling, 1988 Scherer Oshinsky,
    1977) .
  • - In a developmental process, children should be
    able to identify emotional meaning in music as
    soon as they are able to perceive these
    higher-order structural invariants (Cunningham
    Sterling, 1988).
  • - Children as young as four years of age are
    capable of reliably identifying discrete
    categories of emotional meaning in music at
    levels greater than chance. (Cunningham
    Sterling, 1988 Cunningham and Leviton, 1991
    Gentile, Pick, Flom, Campos 1994).
  • - Some gender differences in childrens ability
    to identify emotional meaning in music have been
    reported females have been more accurate in the
    identification of sad and fearful segments
    (Cunningham Sterling, 1988).

4
  • Emotional Understanding of Expressive Body
    Movements/Dance
  • - Anecdotal evidence has suggested that infants
    and children respond to music through movement
    (Moog, 1976).
  • - Infants movements were more lively and
    rhythmic in response to a lively and rhythmic
    segment than to a slow music segment (Trehub,
    1990).
  • - Children as young as 4 years of age can
    nonverbally identify discrete emotional meaning
    in expressive body movements at above-chance
    levels and children as young as five years of
    age show an increased ability to identify such
    emotional meaning in expressive body movements
    and utilize specific body movement to make
    emotion attributions (Boone Cunnigham, 1998).

5
Goals of Phase 1
  • - To determine the ability of preschoolers to
    encode emotional meaning through the medium of
    expressive movement/dance.
  • Hypotheses
  • - Children will encode the emotional meaning of
    the music segments via their videotaped
    dance/expressive movement performances.
  • - Five-year-old children will more accurately
    portray emotional meaning than will four-year-old
    children.
  • - Females may more accurately portray sad and
    fearful categories of emotion than will males.

6
Method
  • Subjects
  • Preschoolers
  • - Twenty-three four year-olds (M46.7 mos) and
    twenty-four five year-olds (M59.7 mos). Both
    age groups had approximately an equal numbers of
    males and females.
  • Adults
  • - Sixty-six raters (M18.0 yrs), balanced for
    gender, were drawn from an introductory
    psychology class and a college-level summer
    program for high school students.
  • Design
  • - A 2(Age) X 2(Gender) X 3(Emotional Category)
    design was employed, with age and gender as
    between-subject factors and emotional
    categorization as a within-subjects factor.

7
Stimuli
  • - Twelve Music Segments, pre-rated as belonging
    to one of four emotional categories - happy, sad,
    angry, and fearful, were used.
  • Four segments, one from each emotion category,
    were used as modeling segments.
  • The remaining eight segments, two from each
    emotion category, were used as test segments.
  • - Each segment was approximately 20 to 30 seconds
    long. Table 1 below provides the pilot
    information obtained on all 12 segments.
  • - Two identical neutral-faced teddy bears were
    utilized as play partners to the preschoolers.

8
Table 1
  • Percentage of Interrater Agreement Among Adult
    Pilot
  • Subjects for the Primary Emotion in Each Music
    Segment
  • Modeling Segments
  • Composition/Composer Emotion
    Percentage Agreement
  • Rumanian Rhapsody, Happiness
    100
  • Opus 11/Enesco
  • Peer Gynt Ase's Death Sadness
    100
  • Suite No. 1 Opus 46/
  • Grieg
  • Theme to Lifeforce/ Anger 55.9
  • Mancini
  • Surprise Attack, Fear 76.5
  • The Wrath ofKhan/
  • Horner

9
  • Test Segments
  • Composition/Composer Emotion Percentage
    Agreement
  • Concerto in D, Opus 35/ Sadness 100
    Tschaikovsky
  • The Humorous Song/ Happiness 94.1
  • Lyadov
  • The Rite of Spring/ Fear 85.3
  • Stravinsky
  • The Red Poppy, Anger 76.5 the Russian
    Sailor's
  • Dance/ Gliere
  • Winter Games/ Happiness 100 Foster
  • Anvil of Crom Anger 88.2
  • Conan/Poledouris
  • Venus/ Holst Sadness 100
  • The Walls Converge, Fear 76.5 Star Wars/
    Williams

10
Procedure
  • Preschoolers
  • - The task was structured as a game. Subjects
    were brought to a quiet area where the equipment
    was set up and told the following
  • - "We are now going to play a game that involves
    music and dancing. This bear, Fuzzy," (holds up
    his/her bear) "likes to dance to music. Your
    bear, Furry, does too." (Experimenter hands the
    second bear to the subject.) "Let me show you
    how to play this game. (Turns on music.
    Experimenter starts to dance. Note the first
    four segments used for modeling were
    approximately twice as long to allow time to
    model the dance behavior.) "Can you hear that
    music? This music has feeling. Can you hear the
    feeling in the music? I can also dance with that
    same feeling. Can you see the feeling in the way
    that I'm dancing? Can you dance that way?"
    (Experimenter allows the subject dance on his or
    her own. If necessary, the experimenter
    replayed the segment until the subject shows an
    appropriate imitative behavior.)

11
  • - (Then as each new modeling segment is
    introduced) "Uh-oh, the music has changed. Did
    you hear that? This music has a different
    feeling. I'm going to have to change the way I'm
    dancing see how I'm dancing. I'm dancing with
    this new feeling. Can you hear that feeling in
    the music? Can you see that feeling in the way
    that I'm dancing? Can you dance that way?
  • - (Then to introduce the testing task) "You know
    what? Furry wants to dance these next eight
    pieces of music with just you. Can you do that?
    Good. Listen to the music." (The subject then
    danced alone with the second bear. Between each
    segment, the experimenter prompted the child to
    dance to the segment, but no evaluative feedback
    was given. Occasionnally the experimenter would
    state that the bear was having fun. After the
    fourth testing segment, the second bear would ask
    to dance alone with the child.)

12
  • Adults
  • - Videotapes of the preschoolers
    dancing/expressively moving the bears were
    created. Segments were remixed and shown to
    adult subjects. Each videotape segment was show
    for the duration of the musical accompaniment and
    lasted approximately 20 to 30 secs.
  • - Adult subjects were used to rate the videotaped
    segments of the preschoolers dance performances.
    Adults viewed the videotaped segments in groups
    of five for a period of one hour. Approximately
    20 of the subjects watched all six videotapes
    the remaining subjects watched only a single
    taped session. ( Results showed that there were
    no differences between the subjects who watched
    all six tapes and the subjects who only watched a
    single tape.) Videotaped segments were presented
    in groups of four, all from the same subject,
    representing one of each of the four target
    emotions. Collectively, each group of four
    segments was shown without interruption, then
    repeated with 10 second pauses to allow the
    raters to evaluate each segment.

13
  • - Raters were asked to categorize each segment
    into one of the four emotions and rate how
    intensely the emotion was being expressed on a
    seven point Likert scale. Raters were told that
    each of the four target emotions was represented
    within each grouping of four performances.
    However, they were also informed that the
    children may not have accurately depicted the
    emotion and that they should answer freely which
    emotion they felt was being expressed within each
    distinct performance. Thus, raters were provided
    with some information to allow discriminant
    categorization, but were also free to answer with
    any emotion for a given performance.

14
Results
  • Accuracy Analysis
  • - Accuracy was measured as a function of how
    often adult raters identified the target emotion
    in each videotaped performance. For any given
    segment, there was a total of 16 raters thus,
    the range of correct identifications for each
    segment was from 0 to 16.
  • - A criterion method was used to identify which
    segments each child accurately portrayed the
    target emotion. With an expected (chance)
    frequency of correct identification by adult
    judges of 4 (25) out of 16, a total of 8 (50)
    or more judges out of 16 categorizing the
    performance as matching the target emotion yields
    a chi-square value of 5.25 with an associated
    probability of 022. Given the low statistical
    sensitivity of the chi-square statistic, such a
    criterion method is actually a more conservative
    method for assessing accuracy.
  • Although judges were presented with an equal
    number of happy, sad, angry, and fearful
    performances, their responses showed a stronger
    preference for happiness (33) and sadness (28)
    than for anger (23) and fear (16). To adjust
    for this bias, these percentages were used to
    estimate the expected (chance) frequency of
    correct identification when calculating the
    adjusted chi-square criteria for each emotion.

15
- Both four-year-olds (M5.35) and and 5-yr-olds
(M6.40) performed above chance (M4.0),
t(22)4.44, prespectively, depicted in Figure 1.
16
The data were subjected to a 2 Child Age X 2
Child Sex X 4 Emotion Analysis of Variance with
repeated measures on the last factor which
revealed significant main effects for Age,
F(1,43)4.72, p.035, depicted in Figure 1, and
Emotion, F(3, 129)21.59, pFigure 2.
17
Goals of Phase 2
  • - To determine if preschool children
    systematically vary their expressive movement
    performances, changing the spatiotemporal cues of
    their movements depending upon which emotion they
    are attempting to encode.
  • Hypotheses
  • - Children will systematically vary the
    spatiotemporal cues of their expressive movement
    performances as a function of the target emotion
    category.
  • - Five-year-old children will show more variation
    than four-year-old children in their encoding of
    the spatiotemporal cues.
  • - There may be some gender differences in the
    manner in which children systematically vary
    their expressive movement performances.

18
Method
  • Coders
  • - Ten coders assessed six targeted behavioral
    cues used by the child actors when enacting the
    expressive movement patterns to music. Each
    coder evaluated all 376 segments for the
    specified behavioral cue.
  • Design
  • - A 2(Age) X 2(Gender) X 3(Emotional Category)
    design was employed, with age and gender as
    between-subject factors and emotional
    categorization as a within-subjects factor.

19
Results
  • Cue Analysis
  • - Each cue was analyzed in a separate 2(Age Group
    of Child) X 2(Sex of Child) X 4(Target Emotion
    Category) analysis of variance with repeated
    measures across all 376 expressive movement
    performances.
  • - There was no main effect for Age Group of Child
    for any of the cues.
  • - There were main effects for Target Emotion
    Category for Force (F(3,129)28.10, pRotation (F(3,129)7.21, pMovement Pattern (F(3,129)13.69, p(F(3,129)38.36, pcues (F(3,129)20.55, pAffect.
  • - There were significantly greater amounts of
    Rotation and faster Tempo in the happy and angry
    performances than the sad and fearful
    performances.
  • - There were significantly greater amounts of
    Force and Upward Movement in the happy and angry
    performances than the fearful performances. Sad
    performances had significantly lower amounts of
    Force and Upward Movement than the fearful
    performances.

20
(No Transcript)
21
  • Cue Analysis (Cont.)
  • - There were significantly lower amounts of
    Shifts in Movement Pattern for the sad
    performances compared to the angry, happy, and
    fearful performances.
  • - There were significant Sex of Child X Target
    Emotion Category interaction effects for Tempo
    (F(3,129)3.24, p.024) and Upward Movement
    analyses (F(3,129)4.02, p.009). While both
    boys and girls showed greater activity for the
    happy and angry performances compared to the sad
    and fearful performances, boys showed greater
    relative activity than the girls with these happy
    and angry performances.
  • - A Discriminant Function Analysis yielded a
    single function which utilized the cue ratings
    from force, rotation, shifts in movement pattern,
    and tempo upward movement loaded on a third
    non-significant function. High values of this
    function predicted a group categorization of
    happiness, moderately high values predicted a
    group categorization of anger, moderately low
    values predicted a group categorization of fear,
    and very low values predicted a group
    categorization of sadness.

22
(No Transcript)
23
(No Transcript)
24
Discussion
  • - These findings demonstrate children as young as
    4- and 5-years-old are able to encode emotional
    meaning through expressive movement,
    approximately the same age at which they begin to
    decode emotional meaning in this and other
    nonverbal media.
  • - These findings provide support for the use of
    nonverbal response formats when studying the
    early recognition of emotional meaning.
  • - These findings suggest a strong intermodal
    connection between music, movement, and emotional
    meaning. These children were able to reliably
    use expressive movement to portray emotional
    meaning in music.
  • - These findings suggest that a more thorough
    investigation of childrens ability to use such
    cues to decode and encode emotional meaning
    across all nonverbal media is warranted.
  • - Aside from some minor differences, there was no
    evidence of a dramatic gender difference in the
    ability to encode emotional meaning via
    expressive body movement.

25
References
  • Boone, R. T., Cunningham, J. G. (1998).
    Childrens decoding of emotion in expressive body
    movement The development of cue attunement.
    Developmental Psychology, 34, 1007-1016.
  • Clynes, M. (ed.). (1982). Music, mind, and
    brain The neurospscyhology of music. New York,
    NY Plenum.
  • Cunningham, J.G., Sterling, R.S. (1988).
    Developmental change in the understanding of
    affective meaning in music. Motivation and
    Emotion, 12, 399-413.
  • Cunningham, J.G., and Leviton, J.M. (1991).
    Preschoolers' understanding of emotional meaning
    in music. Poster presented at the meetings of
    the Society for Research in Child Development,
    Seattle, WA.

26
  • Gentile, D. A., Pick, A. D., Flom, R. A.,
    Campos, J. J. (1994). Adults and preschoolers
    perception of emotional meaning in music. Poster
    presented at 13th Biennial Conference on Human
    Development, Pittsburgh, PA.
  • Moog, H. (1976). The development of musical
    experience in children of preschool age.
    Psychology of Music, 4, 38 - 45.
  • Scherer, K. R., Oshinsky, J. S. (1977). Cue
    utilization in emotion attribution from auditory
    stimulus. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 331 - 346.
  • Trehub, S. (1990). Infant Movement Responses to
    Music. Poster presented at the International
    Conference on Infant Studies, Montreal.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com