Examining Hypermedia as a Means to Improve Access to the General Education Curriculum for Students with Reading Difficulties - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 50
About This Presentation
Title:

Examining Hypermedia as a Means to Improve Access to the General Education Curriculum for Students with Reading Difficulties

Description:

Students with LD comprise the largest subgroup of students served under IDEA ... reading development (Share, McGee, & Silva, 1989; Vellutino, Scanlon, & Lyon, 2000) ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:82
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 51
Provided by: matthew304
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Examining Hypermedia as a Means to Improve Access to the General Education Curriculum for Students with Reading Difficulties


1
Examining Hypermedia as a Means to Improve Access
to the General Education Curriculum for Students
with Reading Difficulties
  • Dr. Matthew T. Marino
  • Washington State University
  • 2007

2
Overview
  • Context for this investigation
  • Students with LD in reading and poor readers
  • Adolescents and expository texts
  • Barriers to the accommodations process
  • Improving reading comprehension
  • Technology in education
  • Can hypermedia improve access to the general
    education curriculum?
  • Current Research

3
Students with Learning Disabilities (LD)
  • Students with LD comprise the largest subgroup of
    students served under IDEA (U.S. Department of
    Education, 2002)
  • The number of students age 12 to 17 identified
    with LD has increased 44 in the past 10 years
    (Lyon et al, 2001)
  • Only 62 of students with LD graduate with a
    diploma (U.S. Department of Education, 2002)
  • Educational costs for students with LD are 1.6
    times that of regular education students (Special
    Education Expenditure Project SEEP, 2003)
  • Students served under IDEA must be provided with
    access to the general education curriculum to the
    greatest extent possible (IDEA, 1997)

Review of Literature
4
LD Identification Process Implications
  • IQ-achievement discrepancy identification is
    changing
  • LD in reading defined
  • IQ does not strongly correlate with reading
    achievement or rate of reading development
    (Share, McGee, Silva, 1989 Vellutino, Scanlon,
    Lyon, 2000)
  • IQ does not predict a students ability to read
    or profit from remediation (Siegel, 1988
    Vellutino et al., 2000)

Review of Literature
5
LD in Reading and Poor Readers
  • Poor readers defined (Vellutino et al., 2000)
  • Poor readers show many of the same
    characteristics as students with LD in reading
    (Fletcher et al., 1994 Stanovich and Siegel,
    1994)
  • Students with LD in reading and poor readers
    possess virtually indistinguishable reading
    growth curves in grades 1 through 12 (Vellutino
    et al., 2000)
  • For the purposes of this study, students with LD
    in reading will be combined with poor readers and
    referred to as students with reading difficulties
    (RD)
  • Students with RD in this study scored below the
    25 on standardized measures of reading
    achievement during the academic year prior to
    this study.

Review of Literature
6
Statement of the Problem
  • More than 12.4 million students experience
    significant difficulties learning to read
    (National Center on Educational Statistics,
    2003).
  • Students with RD are failing to make adequate
    yearly progress in the general education
    curriculum (Lyon et al., 2001 Mastropieri,
    Scruggs, Graetz, 2003 U.S. Department of
    Education, 2003).
  • Traditional methods of instruction (e.g.,
    lectures based on readings in expository texts)
    are not effective for students with RD (Lapp,
    Flood, Ranck-Buhr, 1995 Maccinin, Gagnon,
    Hughes, 2002).
  • The number of students with RD in inclusive
    classrooms is increasing (Lyon et al. 2001).

Review of Literature
7
Adolescents and Expository Texts
  • Expository text defined
  • Students with RD
  • Often lack prior knowledge (Gersten, Fuchs,
    Williams, Baker, 2001)
  • Are unaware of the text structures they are
    reading (Meyer, Brandt, Bluth, 1980)
  • Retrieve information randomly (Wilson Rupley,
    1997)
  • Have difficulty determining essential information
    (Engert Thomas, 1987)
  • Do not utilize text cues (Gersten et al, 2001)
  • Fail to recognize when they are not comprehending
    new information (Gersten et al., 2001)

Review of Literature
8
This Leads To
  • Low levels of reading comprehension (Gersten et
    at., 2001)
  • An inability to formulate questions and
    hypotheses (Wilson Rupley, 1997)
  • Failure to make abstract connections (Engert
    Thomas, 1987)
  • Frustration, lower motivation, expected failure
    (McKinney, Osborne, Schulte, 1993)

What do we do for these students?
Review of Literature
9
Barriers to the Accommodations Process
  • Teachers at the secondary level feel pressured to
    progress through the curriculum (Mastropieri,
    Scruggs, Graetz, 2003)
  • Teachers ability to provide meaningful
    accommodations is hampered by large class sizes
    and a lack of resources (Lancaster, Schumaker,
    Deshler, 2002)
  • Many general education teachers do not have the
    time or expertise to provide meaningful
    accommodations (Mastropieri et al., 2003)

How can we improve this process?
Review of Literature
10
Improving Reading Comprehension
  • Modify instructional materials to match the
    students reading ability (Mastropieri Scruggs,
    2004)
  • Present information using multiple modalities
    (MacArthor, Ferretti, Okolo, Cavalier, 2001)
  • Offer repeated reading and practice opportunities
    (Jenkins, Stein, Wysocki, 1984 Bryant, 2003)
  • Provide students with opportunities to gain
    additional background knowledge quickly
    (MacArthor et al., 2001)

How can we do this?
Review of Literature
11
Consider Technology
  • Current policy calls for the increased use of
    technology to support student learning (U.S.
    Department of Education, 2003)
  • Technology use in regular education classrooms is
    rapidly increasing (U.S. Department of Education,
    2003 Vannatta OBannon, 2002 )
  • Technology may improve access to the general
    education curriculum (Pucket, 2004 Behrmann
    Jerome, 2002 Edyburn, 2002 Fisher Frey, 2001)
  • Technology should be used in concert with other
    instructional methods (e.g., classroom discourse)
    (Yerrick, 2000 De La Paz MacArthor, 2003
    Fuchs, Fuchs, Kazdan, 1999)

Review of Literature
12
Can Technology Improve Access to the General
Education Curriculum?
  • Hypermedia defined
  • Hypermedia may eliminate the overuse of
    expository texts (Lancaster et al., 2002)
  • Hypermedia provides
  • Information on demand (McKenna, Reinking, Labbo,
    Keiffer, 1999)
  • Tools that support cognitive processes (Lajoie,
    1993)
  • Built-in accommodations by allowing teachers to
    modify task difficulty and select appropriate
    readability levels (Edyburn, 2000 Behrmann
    Jerome, 2002 Pucket, 2004)

Review of Literature
13
Hypermedia Research
  • Hypermedia allows teachers to monitor student
    progress and resource use (McKenna et al., 1999)
  • Students and teachers report positive outcomes
    from using hypermedia (Lancaster et al.,, 2002
    Garthwait, 2004 Lewis, 2000)
  • There are a limited number of studies examining
    the efficacy of hypermedia as a means of
    improving comprehension for students with RD
    (Maccini, Gagnon, Hughes, 2002)
  • Research with students in regular education
    classrooms is promising, but inconclusive
    (Oliver, 1999 Land, 2000 Liu, 2004)

Review of Literature
14
Need for Current Research
  • Preliminary research examining the effects of
    technology-based textual modifications (e.g.,
    readability levels within hypermedia programs) is
    inconclusive (MacArthur et al., 2001).
  • Research is needed to determine the types of
    comprehension instruction that are most
    beneficial for low ability readers in secondary
    content area courses (Report of the National
    Reading Panel, 2000).
  • Additional research is needed to determine which
    cognitive tools are most beneficial to students
    with RD in middle school science classes (Liu,
    2004).

Review of Literature
15
Theoretical framework
  • Knowledge Construction Through Conceptual Change
    (KCTCC) expands on the theory of Schema Change
    (Winn, 2004).
  • Perpetual cycle of learning where prior knowledge
    directs how individuals seek, identify, and
    interpret information (Neisser, 1976).
  • The way in which individuals construct knowledge
    can not be predicted or supported using teacher
    directed instructional designs (Duffy Jonassen,
    1992).
  • KCTCC is most effective in problem-based learning
    environments that incorporate technology (Winn,
    2004).

Review of Literature
16
Universal Design for Learning (UDL)
  • UDL supports KCTCC by
  • Assuming students enter learning experiences with
    varying degrees of prior knowledge (Hitchcock,
    Meyer, Rose, Jackson, 2002).
  • Using technology to incorporate video clips,
    graphic organizers, illustrations, and text
    modifications into the curriculum (Mastropieri,
    Scruggs, Bakken, Whedon, 1996).
  • Allowing learners to access information quickly
    using multiple modalities (MacArthur, Ferretti,
    Okolo, Cavalier, 2001).

Review of Literature
17
Why Middle School Science?
  • Science is one of the most difficult subjects for
    students with RD to learn due to its complex
    vocabulary and theoretical nature (Mastropieri et
    al., 2003).
  • Students with RD in middle school science classes
    typically read at the 4th or 5th grade level
    (Mastropieri et al., 2003).
  • If textual modifications and cognitive tools can
    improve access to the middle school science
    curriculum, their application in other content
    areas may yield similar results.

Review of Literature
18
Research Purpose
  • Statement of the research problem
  • There is a need to determine whether hypermedia
    can improve access to the general education
    curriculum for students with RD and low ability
    readers (Lyon and Moats, 1997 MacArthur et al.,
    2001 National Reading Panel, 2000).
  • For the purposes of this study
  • Low ability readers are defined as students
    scoring lt 50 on the Degrees of Reading Power
    (DRP) subtest of the Connecticut Mastery Test.
    The DRP is a norm-referenced measure of reading
    achievement (Touchstone Applied Science
    Associates, 2004).
  • Students with reading difficulties (RD) are
    defined as scoring lt 25 on the DRP.

Research Questions
19
Research Questions
  • Are there posttest differences on the science
    posttest and solutions forms measure between
    students with RD (lt25th percentile) and those who
    are not RD (25th - 50th percentile)?
  • Are there posttest differences on the science
    posttest and solution forms measure between
    students who participated in the 4th grade
    readability condition and those who participated
    in the 8th grade readability condition?
  • Is there an interaction between reading ability
    (i.e., students lt25th percentile and students in
    the 26th - 50th percentile) and treatment
    condition (4th grade readability and 8th grade
    readability)? If so, what is the nature of the
    interaction?
  • Is there a relationship between students reading
    ability, use of cognitive tools, and their
    comprehension of scientific concepts and
    processes as measured on the posttest and
    solutions form measure? If so, what is the nature
    of the relationship?

Research Questions
20
Research Design

Methods Design
21
Threats to Validity

Methods Design
22
Setting
  • Four middle schools in New England volunteered to
    participate in the study
  • Schools were selected based on 1) administrative
    consent, 2) 100 teacher agreement to
    participate, and 3) available technology
    resources

Setting Participants
23
Student Demographic Data
  • 50 male, 50 female
  • 87 White, 5 African American, 4 Asian, 4
    Hispanic
  • 89 have a computer at home they can use
  • 54 have a personal computer
  • Response to the statement
  • I am good at using computers
  • 32 strongly agree
  • 62 agree
  • 5 disagree
  • 1 strongly disagree

Setting Participants
24
Participants
  • Students (N 1153) were grouped based on 2004
    DRP scores
  • Group 1 - Students with RD ( lt 25 on DRP)
  • Group 2 - Poor readers (26 - 50 on DRP)
  • Group 3 - Proficient readers ( gt 50 on DRP)
  • Students from groups 1 (n 113) and 2 (n 189)
    were randomly assigned at the student level
    within each class to either the 4th grade
    readability or 8th grade readability condition.
  • Students in group 3 (n 851) received text from
    the program at the 8th grade level

Setting Participants
25
The Intervention Alien Rescue

Instrumentation
26
Performance Measures
  • Pre/Posttest
  • 25 item paper pencil multiple choice test
  • Reliability of .85 established in previous study
    (Pedersen Williams, 2004)
  • 19 items assess knowledge and comprehension
    (e.g., A world would have a magnetic field
    if________ )
  • 6 items assess students ability to apply
    knowledge (e.g., Scientist want to measure Mars
    atmosphere. What instrument would they use ______
    ?)
  • Six solution forms
  • One paper and pencil solution form for each alien
    species
  • Two column form that requires students to
    analyze, synthesize, and evaluate data from the
    Alien Rescue program
  • Scaffolds the learning process through prompts
    (e.g., magnetic field)
  • Contains open-ended and narrative response items
  • Piloted with 300 students (Marino, 2005)
  • Established inter-rater reliability of .90

Instrumentation
27
Procedures

Methods
28
Preliminary Analyses
  • Students who were absent more than 3 days (20)
    of the intervention were excluded from analyses.
  • Independent samples t-test results, t (302)
    1.22, p .22, d .01 indicated that there were
    no significant differences (DRP group 1 vs. 2) on
    the pretest measure.
  • Principal component factor analysis of posttest
    measures (posttest solutions forms) indicates
    68 of variability was explained by posttest.
  • Solutions forms grouped into one subscale (total
    score) with one factor solution reliability of
    .94.

Analysis
29
Analysis Research questions 1 - 3
  • Compute descriptive statistics
  • Separate two-way ANOVAs
  • DRP group (DRP group 1 vs. DRP group 2)
    and treatment condition (4th grade text vs.
    8th grade text) as between-subjects
    independent variables. Posttest and combined
    solutions forms scores (total score) as
    dependent variables.

Analysis Results
30
Results Research Questions 1 - 3
  • RQ1 Differences by DRP group
  • Results of the ANOVA for the posttest were not
    significant
  • F (1, 298) 3.552, p .060, d 0.01
  • Results of the ANOVA for total score were
    significant
  • F (1, 281) 3.974, p .047, d 0.01
  • RQ2 Differences by treatment (4th vs. 8th grade
    text)
  • Results of the ANOVA for the posttest were not
    significant
  • F (1, 298) 0.369, p .554, d .001.
  • Results of the ANOVA for total score were not
    significant
  • F (1, 281) 0.03, p .872, d lt .001.
  • RQ3 Interaction between treatment and DRP group
  • Results of the ANOVA for posttest were not
    significant
  • F (1, 298) 2.56, p .111, d .008
  • Results of the ANOVA for total score were not
    significant
  • F (1, 281) 1.28, p .259, d .004

Results
31
Student Learning
  • Paired samples t-test (pre/posttest) with DRP
    groups collapsed was significant (t 25.719,
  • p lt .001, d 1.5)
  • Posttest mean was 14.22 (56) on a 25-point scale
  • On Solutions Forms (216-point scale)
  • DRP 1 mean score 142.05 (66)
  • DRP 2 mean score 151.77 (70)

Did they use the built-in cognitive tools?
Results
32
Analysis Research Question 4
  • Group cognitive tools into four categories
    (Lajoie, 1993 Liu, 2004 Liu Bera, 2005)
  • Tools that share cognitive overload (e.g.,
    databases)
  • Tools that support cognitive process (e.g., field
    journal)
  • Tools that support out-of-reach activities (e.g.,
    astroengineering)
  • Tools that support hypothesis testing (e.g.,
    telemetry)
  • Obtain correlations between students use of
    tools and performance on posttest and total score
  • Separate one-way ANOVAs with tool use category as
    the dependent variable and DRP group as
    independent variable.
  • Simultaneous multiple linear regression analysis
    to determine how tool use (by category) predicts
    performance on posttest and total score measures

Analysis Results
33
Results Research Question 4
  • The strongest correlation was between tools that
    share cognitive overload and the posttest (r
    .224).
  • ANOVA (tool use DV, DRP IV) was significant for
    each of the four tool categories.
  • Cog. overload F (2, 954) 12.60, p lt .001,
    d .03
  • Cog. process F (2, 954) 4.86, p
    .008, d .01
  • Out-of-reach F (2, 954) 3.07, p
    .047, d .006
  • Hypothesis F (2, 954) 5.57, p
    .004, d .01
  • Scheffes post hoc analysis indicates significant
    differences between students by DRP groups

Analysis Results
34
Results of Multiple Regression - RQ4
  • Multiple regression indicates main effects for
    tools that share cognitive overload and tools
    that support out-of-reach activities on posttest
    scores.
  • A significant interaction between DRP group and
    tools that support cognitive overload was present
    on the posttest regression analysis.
  • A significant interaction between DRP group and
    hypothesis testing was present on the solution
    form regression analysis.

What does this tell us?
Analysis Results
35
Interpreting the Multiple Regression Analysis
  • On the Posttest Regression Analysis
  • For every unit increase in the SQRT of cognitive
    overload
  • We predict a .792 unit increase in posttest score
    for students in DRP groups 1 2.
  • We predict a .33 unit increase for students in
    DRP group 3.
  • For every unit increase in the SQRT of tools that
    support out-of-reach activities students scores
    on the posttest decrease by .674 units.
  • On the Solutions Form Regression Analysis
  • Students in DRP 3 gained 7.886 units for each
    unit gain using tools that support hypothesis
    testing.
  • Students in DRP groups 1 2 gained 1.264 units
    for each unit gain using tools that support
    hypothesis testing.

Analysis Results
36
Discussion
  • Students in DRP groups 1 and 2 (lt50) performed
    in similar ways during the intervention.
  • There was a lack of treatment effect due to
    readability level of text.
  • Students used cognitive tools differently
    depending on their reading ability level.

Discussion
37
Implications
  • Students need systematic, explicit, scaffolded
    instruction to utilize cognitive tools (Gersten
    Baker, 1998)
  • Opportunities for students to reflect on and have
    immediate feedback regarding tool use (Swanson
    Hoskyn, 1998)
  • Inclusion of tools that support cognitive
    overload and out-of-reach activities (Land, 2000
    Liu, 2004 Williams Peterson, 2004)
  • Technology and content area training for special
    education teachers (Sharpe Hawes, 2003)
  • Increased collaboration between special education
    and regular education teachers (Moore Keefe,
    2001)
  • Improved instructional strategies for general
    education teachers (Washburn-Moses, 2005)

Implications
38
Limitations
  • Paper and pencil assessments
  • Levels and types of classroom discourse
  • Tool use data highly skewed
  • Student differences with problem-based learning
  • Sample (SES, representation of minority
    populations)
  • Teacher effects

Limitations
39
Questions?

40
Links
  • Descriptives by DRP group
  • Descriptives by Treatment Condition
  • Two-Way ANOVA Posttest
  • Two-Way ANOVA Total Score
  • Tool Category Correlations
  • Posttest Multiple Regression
  • Total Score Multiple Regression
  • Scheffes Post Hoc for Tool Use
  • Threats to Validity
  • Cell sizes for low ability readers
  • Teacher Effects
  • Research Questions

41
Descriptive Statistics by Reading Ability

Maximum Posttest Score 25 Maximum Total Score
216
RQ 1 -3
Results
42
Descriptive Statistics by Treatment Condition

Maximum Posttest Score 25 Maximum Total Score
216
RQ 1 - 3
Results
43
Posttest Two-Way ANOVA

RQ 1 -3
Results
44
Total Score Two-Way ANOVA

RQ 1 - 3
Results
45
Tool Category Correlations

RQ 4
Results
46
Scheffes Post Hoc Analysis for Tool Use

RQ 4
Results
47
Multiple Regression Posttest

MR 4
Results
48
Multiple Regression Total Score

MR 4
Results
49
Teacher Effects
  • Results of one-way ANOVA with posttest and
    solution form scores as dependent variables and
    Teacher as the independent variable were
    significant.
  • Posttest
  • F (15, 1190) 16.131,
  • p lt .001, d 0.17
  • Solutions Forms
  • F (15, 1153) 112.334,
  • p lt .001, d .59

Results
50
Cell sizes for low ability readers

Group 1 Group 2
Text presented at 4th grade level
Text presented at 8th grade level
Power analysis indicates optimum group sizes of
68 (f .20, d .57) to achieve a power of .80.
Methods
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com